Making Sense - Sam Harris - April 10, 2020


#196 — The Science of Happiness


Episode Stats

Length

59 minutes

Words per Minute

206.41095

Word Count

12,312

Sentence Count

6

Misogynist Sentences

4

Hate Speech Sentences

3


Summary

Laurie Santos is a professor of psychology at Yale University and teaches the most popular course at Yale, Psychology and the Good Life. She also runs the comparative cognition lab and the canine cognition center at Yale. In this episode, we talk about the role of expectations and the experiencing self versus the remembered self, the importance of social connections, the effect of focusing on the happiness of others as opposed to one s own introversion versus extroversion, the influence of technology on our social lives, our relationship to time, the connection between happiness and ethics, hedonic adaptation, the power of mindfulness, resilience, the often illusory significance of reaching one s goals, and other topics. I really enjoyed this episode and hope you find it useful. Make sense of it! -Sam Harris If you can t afford a subscription, there's an option at Samharris.org to request a free account and we'll grant 100 of those requests, no questions asked. You'll need to subscribe to our private RSS feed to get access to all the latest episodes of the Making Sense Podcast, where you'll get 100% of the podcast's episodes, plus access to the latest podcasts, books, videos, and podcasts, plus all kinds of awesome stuff. Samarris is making sense of happiness, happiness, and well-being. Want to become a supporter of Making Sense? Subscribe to Making Sense here: bit.ly/support-a-santos/making-sense/p&t=1&ref=a&q&q=A&q_t&qid=3&q is a must-listen to make sense? Thanks for listening to the podcast? Thank you for supporting the podcast, Samarie, I hope you'll find out more about the podcast and keep listening to this podcast! -Jon Soranos, Jonestro? --Jonestros -- ? This is a post on the podcast by Jonestros? Jon is a fellow? "Jonestro , . "The Happiness is the most important part of this podcast by me? ? " & ) ! And ... (A must-be a good one? , "A good friend of the show?" : and so on and so much more?


Transcript

00:00:00.000 welcome to the making sense podcast this is sam harris just a note to say that if you're hearing
00:00:14.540 this you're not currently on our subscriber feed and we'll only be hearing partial episodes of the
00:00:19.580 podcast if you'd like access to full episodes you'll need to subscribe at samharris.org
00:00:25.060 there you'll find our private rss feed to add to your favorite podcatcher along with other
00:00:30.220 subscriber only content and as always i never want money to be the reason why someone can't
00:00:35.200 listen to the podcast so if you can't afford a subscription there's an option at samharris.org
00:00:40.060 to request a free account and we grant 100 of those requests no questions asked today i'm speaking with
00:00:47.560 laurie santos laurie is a professor of psychology at yale university and she hosts the very popular
00:00:54.220 podcast the happiness lab and she teaches the most popular course at yale which is on the
00:01:00.720 scientific understanding of happiness she also runs the comparative cognition laboratory and the canine
00:01:05.760 cognition center at yale and here we get into what we know or at least have good reason to believe
00:01:13.040 scientifically about the causes and conditions of happiness at this point we talk about the role
00:01:20.620 of expectations and the experiencing self versus the remembered self we talk about framing effects
00:01:28.160 and the importance of social connections the effect of focusing on the happiness of others as opposed to
00:01:34.560 one's own introversion versus extroversion the influence of technology on our social lives our
00:01:42.060 relationship to time the connection between happiness and ethics hedonic adaptation the power of
00:01:49.260 mindfulness resilience the often illusory significance of reaching one's goals
00:01:55.460 and other topics anyway i really enjoyed this i hope you find it useful i now bring you laurie santos
00:02:04.460 i am here with laurie santos laurie thanks for joining me thanks for having me on the show so uh this
00:02:15.640 was a long time coming many people wanted to hear from you how do you describe what it is you do
00:02:20.900 academically and intellectually yeah so i am a professor of psychology here at yale university
00:02:27.540 my day job as a psychologist is is involved in studying what makes the human mind special
00:02:33.640 and i do that by studying non-human primates and domesticated dogs but but most of my time these days
00:02:40.160 is taken up with a different scientific pursuit in psychology i've become super interested in the
00:02:44.620 scientific basis of happiness and well-being and you you have a podcast titled the happiness lab
00:02:50.500 where you go into these issues in depth and the course you teach at yale am i right in thinking this
00:02:57.840 is the the most popular course at the university yeah so in 2018 i taught a new class on this topic
00:03:04.000 called psychology and the good life and the first time i taught it it did become yale's largest class
00:03:08.760 ever just under like 1200 students enrolled which was about one out of every four students at yale
00:03:14.100 since then we put the class online on coursera.org and it's now one of coursera's biggest classes
00:03:19.800 and just in the last month we've had over a million learners enroll wow well that's great happiness really
00:03:26.780 is a paramount concern for everyone whether they think about it in those terms or not let's just focus
00:03:33.680 on the word for a second because happiness at least in english is a somewhat insubstantial concept and
00:03:42.200 people will often say something like you know there's much more to life than happiness mere happiness
00:03:49.560 sounds like a somewhat a feat goal or primary value it seems to grade into something like hedonism
00:03:58.240 or pleasure and then people would will tend to try to balance that in their talk about the goals to
00:04:05.960 which human life could tend with concepts like meaning and virtue and then many of us find ourselves
00:04:12.740 using a word like flourishing which is strangely stilted although not as stilted as using the greek
00:04:19.540 eudaimonia and then i tend to talk about well-being a lot and you actually just use that term
00:04:24.700 so how do you think about the concept i mean mostly i just think i wish we had better terms
00:04:29.420 than that everyone agreed on them so i didn't spend a lot of my time kind of fighting about them
00:04:33.240 i use the term happiness because i think that's what a lot of people think of when they're thinking
00:04:38.900 about concepts like well-being and flourishing i agree that happiness is a much more loaded thing
00:04:43.200 because some people think it's about hedonism and really basic kinds of forms of happiness but i think
00:04:48.300 people kind of get this concept of happiness you know we know it from like the declaration of
00:04:51.940 independence right you know life liberty and the pursuit of happiness right but but scientifically
00:04:56.440 speaking i think social scientists mean a particular thing when they use the term happiness
00:05:00.200 or well-being and this is the definition that i end up using in the course which is that you can
00:05:05.100 basically say you're happy if you have a lot of well-being in your life and for your life and and
00:05:11.080 what we mean by that is that kind of happiness in your life is the sort of you know almost hedonistic
00:05:15.640 kind of positive emotion type stuff right you're happy in your life if you have lots of you know
00:05:20.120 positive emotions and laughter and so on and and not many negative emotions like relatively speaking
00:05:24.700 there's not a tremendous amount of sadness and anger although we can debate about how much of
00:05:28.800 that you want but that's kind of being happy in your life but but there's another feature i think that
00:05:33.240 the social scientists really care about and that's that you're happy with your life and so that's
00:05:37.800 basically your answer to the question all things considered how satisfied are you with your life
00:05:41.860 right now and so i think there are these interesting moments where those dissociate right i have my
00:05:46.960 my academic dean here in my residential college you know just had a newborn baby and you know i think
00:05:52.200 she's very satisfied with her life but in her life right now there's a lot of negative emotions of
00:05:56.500 like you know cleaning dirty diapers and not sleeping and these kinds of things and i think you know i see
00:06:01.160 a lot you know when i go to different talks and things of people you know who are really happy in
00:06:05.840 their life you know they have a lot of hedonistic pleasure but really at their core they're really
00:06:09.840 dissatisfied with their life and so i think i think if you're if in my view if you're able to
00:06:15.000 maximize both of those things you know that winds up encompassing things like flourishing and meeting
00:06:20.000 and all these kind of lesser concepts i think if you're happy in your life and with your life you're
00:06:24.300 doing pretty well right was it that distinction happy in your life and happy with your life to my ear
00:06:31.120 that is more or less identical to danny kahneman's distinction between the experiencing and remembering
00:06:37.720 self is there any daylight between these concepts for you or is that the same division i think there's a
00:06:44.300 little dissociation there i think i think you can have happiness in your life and with your life in
00:06:49.320 the experienced self right and so just as an example you know right now i'm you know experiencing lots of
00:06:54.800 positive emotions just from you know daily things i do and daily activities but i also have a lot of
00:06:59.160 meaning from this happiness work and that feels like it right now like i don't have i have to think
00:07:03.820 back on it you know my it's not my future self kind of looking back and thinking like oh i you know
00:07:08.780 that was the kind of thing i really wanted to enjoy i can experience that life satisfaction in the
00:07:12.680 moment and so i think you can actually have both in the experienced self rather than the remembered
00:07:17.860 self although i it sounds to me what you're doing is something i do naturally and this is a point of
00:07:24.300 disagreement between me and danny i really do think the remembering self is simply the experiencing
00:07:31.040 self in one of its modes it feels like something to have these moments of retrospection when asked
00:07:37.040 what story can you tell about your life how satisfied are you the fact that you know in his
00:07:42.780 paradigm he's able to show that there's a mismatch rather often between who you're talking to when
00:07:49.040 you're you're asking about a retrospective judgment and who you're talking to when you're asking for a
00:07:54.580 moment-to-moment accounting of just how you know what it's like to be you still there really is just
00:08:00.200 a single timeline of life experience and as you say the global assessment of one's life is what i'm
00:08:10.400 doing today actually meaningful is it bringing value to the world are the sacrifices that i'm making or the
00:08:16.940 stress i'm under now is it aimed at some purpose that i feel inspired by and that others feel inspired
00:08:24.820 by i mean all of that is that's where the this remembering self and the experiencing self just
00:08:30.240 in my experience they become indistinguishable and so i wonder if you're just taking however
00:08:36.740 inadvertently my side of the argument against danny here that really if we become very fine-grained about
00:08:42.200 what we mean by the experiencing self it just swallows the remembering self yeah i mean i think you're
00:08:49.700 right on this one and i don't know i think i mean i haven't pushed danny on this directly but
00:08:53.200 my sense is that we don't have what the timeline is for the remembered self right you know if any
00:08:58.520 form of retro if any form of meta-analysis of it is the remember itself as soon as you ask me like
00:09:04.000 hey how's things going how satisfied with you if i ever have to take a global view it's possible that
00:09:09.320 that kind of it's using the mechanisms that i use for the remember itself to some interesting extent
00:09:13.660 right i don't think danny's really specified how far back we have to do the remembering but it might
00:09:18.700 be that any any point where we're kind of going meta and thinking about our own happiness
00:09:22.360 might be partly the remembered self and i think this actually brings up a bigger issue with a lot
00:09:26.800 of the happiness research right is that we want to get at what happiness feels like in the moment
00:09:32.080 but the only way we can do that is to ask people and it's very possible that between the experiencing
00:09:37.700 and the asking in any form we're kind of getting some interesting mismatches there like it could be
00:09:42.780 that just having you reflect on your own positive emotions it's going to change that right that might be
00:09:47.040 different than kind of what i was noticing and what i was experiencing and the sum total of that
00:09:51.180 throughout my day which sucks for happiness researchers right because we have to ask people
00:09:55.000 somehow you know i wish there was a thermometer where we could get at happiness or well-being
00:09:59.140 accurately without asking people but we don't really have that and and we don't it's hard for us to ever
00:10:04.620 know if the act of reporting on your happiness is changing it whether that be what you're experiencing
00:10:09.920 what you're remembering in whatever form yeah i have another question here which relates to this which
00:10:16.140 is the role that expectation plays in determining a person's sense of well-being and with expectation i'm
00:10:23.740 thinking it can also be retrospective right so you know i'm having a certain experience it has a
00:10:30.660 a certain emotional valence which could be negative right it could be stressful but because of my
00:10:37.840 expectations or because of how i can even retrospectively reconceive the stress i was just under or i'm
00:10:46.720 currently under this kind of framing effect can seem to in part or fully determine whether an experience
00:10:56.040 gets scored as pure suffering or one of the highlights of my life let's say you're climbing everest
00:11:02.900 right and you know obviously the physical experience is just more or less a pure ordeal but if you get
00:11:10.080 to the top and you get back down without dying and you don't destroy your ethical code by passing
00:11:15.200 somebody's near corpse on the way down we've all heard those horrible stories so you can have something
00:11:21.640 that if you were sampling each time point along the way just looks like torture and yet retrospectively
00:11:28.360 and for real you know for the rest of one's life it's going to seem like one of the best things
00:11:33.440 you've ever done how do you think about those framing effects oh i think those framing effects
00:11:38.020 are huge i mean i almost think that the way you're framing an experience and i mean that in a variety
00:11:43.320 of ways how you're categorizing it how you frame it retrospectively the expectations you have about it
00:11:48.560 going into it i think that those expectations and those categorizations are more powerful in some
00:11:53.860 cases than the actual experience itself for what what we go through i mean you know just there's so
00:11:59.620 many kinds of cases like this so so take you know really classic work in the history of psychology where
00:12:04.800 you give people a particular physiological response and then them give and give them different kinds
00:12:10.840 of frames for how they make sense of it you know so this was back in the day kind of before ethics
00:12:15.180 and social psychology but you basically unknowingly pump subjects full of like adrenaline
00:12:19.740 basically you could basically give them speed without them realizing it and then you you set
00:12:24.220 a frame for what they could be experiencing they're either in a in a room with other subjects who are
00:12:28.760 acting really aggressively who are really angry or who are kind of partying you know they think the
00:12:32.760 experiment's super fun and they're enjoying it and what you find is that the subject's entire
00:12:36.500 experience of that event depends on the frame of the other people around them you know if they're in a
00:12:40.800 room of people who are partying and they're experiencing these physical sensations that are kind of a little bit
00:12:45.740 you know agitated they think it's really fun whereas if they're around other angry people
00:12:49.500 they find it incredibly negative and they see it as angering and so what this shows us is our the
00:12:54.480 basic physiology of what we're experiencing how we actually feel about it whether that's positive
00:12:59.680 or negatively valenced or whether it's something that might lead to happiness or lead to sadness or
00:13:03.800 anger it's completely based on what our expectations are about that moment in some cases the social
00:13:08.400 contagion of other people's expectations about that moment i mean that's the basic physiology and the
00:13:13.660 example you're getting into is even more complicated you know it's not just a single physiological
00:13:18.220 experience in a moment it's integrating across a whole host of physiological experiences and then
00:13:23.660 looking back on it and so we might have some frame about those you know experiences say before we start
00:13:28.720 on mount everest maybe it's a dream of ours and something we've trained for and so on you know that's
00:13:32.880 going to cause us in the moment to see those actual experiences you know tiredness and you know
00:13:37.500 physiological stress and you know fight or flight response all that stuff like we're going to see
00:13:41.760 those differently and then see them differently retrospectively so so i think it's kind of a mess but
00:13:46.340 in some ways i think that's really powerful though right that means that we actually have the chance
00:13:51.740 to reframe things in our life in these powerful ways right and i think the ancient traditions figured
00:13:57.020 that out and then you know the kahneman and tverskies of the world figured that out in modern times
00:14:01.320 and that's exciting because it means we can use these framing techniques to change around our experience we
00:14:06.500 don't actually have to change our physiology to change whether or not some experience makes us happy or sad
00:14:11.440 right and we we actually don't even have to change the past or have avoided certain negative experiences
00:14:19.720 in the past if we can reframe them in the future and this is the one thing that i pulled from existentialism
00:14:26.860 apart from an appreciation for how much of it didn't make sense it's just the sense that you you are always
00:14:33.360 free to tell yourself a new story about the past so if this humiliating failure that has bothered you
00:14:40.900 up until yesterday can be reframed as the thing that caused you to get the tools that are now you
00:14:47.520 know integral to your success or whatever it is you can actually just change your relationship to
00:14:52.180 something that used to be a source of suffering for you and in that sense reach into the past and put
00:14:57.740 it to some order and the most amazing thing about the human mind is that we can do that prospectively
00:15:02.840 too you know there's lovely work by social psychologists like ethan cross that talk about the power of
00:15:08.060 psychological distancing basically trying to think about an event as your future self would think
00:15:13.220 about the event you know so i'm about to go through you know i don't know like a really i'm going to
00:15:17.100 have a really stressful interview with sam harris right before i start that i could think well how
00:15:21.160 would future laurie want to think about this interview i'd want to think you know we had this
00:15:24.400 great discussion and we you know dealt with these hard-hitting issues and you know this is this is going
00:15:28.880 to be awesome and that when i if i were to think that way even before i started the interview
00:15:33.000 it would frame how the conversation was going you know if i kind of got stressed out in the middle of it
00:15:37.000 you know i'd think like oh this is the hard-hitting part that i really wanted to experience you know
00:15:40.620 and it would kind of feel better later and so you know what ethan's shown is that we don't have to
00:15:45.200 just wait till we get to the future to think back retrospectively in this positive way we can use that
00:15:50.500 as a frame in the present to shape experiences over time too and and he's shown that you can do that
00:15:56.100 simply by you know having a narrative in your own head that uses your future self in the third person
00:16:01.440 you know laurie in the future will want to think this way about you know x y and z experience
00:16:05.360 that can shape it in time in real time as you experience the event well lucky for you you're
00:16:10.740 speaking to future sam who's a real pushover so no problem there so let's start from some kind of
00:16:18.620 ground zero for people psychologically so let's imagine someone comes to you you're you're a happiness
00:16:25.800 expert and they come to you and they say that they are profoundly unhappy in their life
00:16:33.400 what generic advice would you give to a person that really is generic that you think is more or
00:16:40.580 less a good idea for virtually anyone you know barring some strange contraindication what do you
00:16:47.740 recommend to people as a first pass for turning the various dials within reach to improve their sense of
00:16:55.560 well-being well i think the first piece of advice is just that the science suggests you can intervene
00:16:59.740 right i mean i think a lot of people who are not happy at a given time think that there's something
00:17:05.380 about them that's messed up right you know genetically they're just predisposed to be unhappy
00:17:09.680 or they're kind of built to be that way and i think you know the first thing to tell people is just that
00:17:14.600 you know that might be the case to a certain extent you know there's some heritability to most
00:17:18.500 well-being measures but there's a lot you can do to intervene on them and so i think that's kind of
00:17:23.280 message number one is look you can take some action and fix this in terms of the specific actions i would
00:17:29.060 suggest you know if you look at the positive psychology literature one of the hugest effects
00:17:34.040 on our own happiness is our social connection you know there's a famous paper by marty seligman and
00:17:39.360 ed diener that said suggests that social social relationships and strong social relationships are
00:17:45.000 necessary for happiness they're not sufficient for happiness but you can't find happy people that
00:17:50.280 don't have them and so that really suggests that if you want to be like happy people you should focus
00:17:55.080 on your social relationships and that means you know taking a hard look at your priorities to figure
00:18:00.100 out if those social relationships are falling by the wayside and i think in the modern day where we
00:18:05.120 prioritize work and the things that go with work and for my students you know their academic
00:18:10.080 performance often that's coming at the opportunity cost of the time you'd spend on your social
00:18:15.020 relationships and so so that's kind of hit number one is are you making time for the people that you really care
00:18:20.900 about in life and and and a lot of that work also comes from some lovely studies by robert waldinger
00:18:26.460 and his colleagues he's part of this long-running harvard happiness study that it's it's super cool
00:18:31.700 it's been studying it's been studying men from harvard and they were men because the studies started back
00:18:37.000 in the 1930s so men from harvard and also like you know men from lower income boston neighborhoods
00:18:42.620 and they've been tracking them over time and so now their original cohort is in their 90s well and
00:18:47.740 they've been able to look at all kinds of features about their health you know their immune function
00:18:50.900 and whether they get heart disease and diabetes and so on and what's remarkable is that a major
00:18:55.960 predictor not just of mental health like happiness but also physical health is the nature of these men's
00:19:02.280 social relationships they're actually predicting longevity now so that the men who are still alive in
00:19:07.240 this cohort study are the ones that seem to have tended to have the best social relationships and so
00:19:12.380 again kind of doubling down on social relationships is great because it's like doing double duty it's not just
00:19:17.480 good for your mental health it's great for your physical health too right yeah so let's run through
00:19:22.000 the the list of things that come to mind and then i'll come back to some of them because i do want to
00:19:26.660 talk about relationships more yeah so so social connection is a big one being other oriented generally
00:19:32.540 and what i mean by this is the the act of paying attention to other people over yourself so being the
00:19:38.460 kind of person that gives to charity that volunteers your time that that is focused on other people
00:19:43.240 generally seems to be a big one for your your happiness and your health one that sounds silly
00:19:48.020 is the what i tell my students are just healthy habits and by that i mean you know the stuff that
00:19:53.300 we know is good for physical health like making sure you're getting enough sleep making sure you're
00:19:56.780 getting enough exercise making sure you're eating right those physical things seem to have a huge
00:20:01.420 impact on people's mental health much more than we think and then i think there's a whole set of
00:20:05.940 things that are more in in your wheelhouse i know the act of being a little bit more present being
00:20:10.620 mindful and then changing your mindset towards things like having a mindset that's a little bit
00:20:15.360 more grateful and a little bit more compassionate generally and these are mindsets that often come
00:20:21.060 from ancient practices like different forms of meditation and so on so so those would be those
00:20:26.180 would be my top hits we can get into the lesser top hits too like right like being being religious
00:20:31.120 it turns out is actually pretty good for your happiness or at least i should clarify not necessarily
00:20:36.340 believing religious believing in religious doctrine but actually taking part in religious practices
00:20:41.280 it turns out is correlated with happiness and yeah we can go way down the list but we'll see we'll see
00:20:46.580 which ones you want to pick up on yeah okay well let's go back to relationships and i think you know
00:20:51.440 knowing something about your work it's not just one's close relationships it's also one's orientation
00:20:56.980 toward strangers i mean whether you will just talk to people in public on an airplane or in line or
00:21:03.280 this is something you've covered in at least one of your podcasts so this would suggest however that
00:21:09.780 extroverts could be at some kind of advantage here and that shyness could be a real impediment to
00:21:17.500 self-actualization of some kind how do you think about that let's leave aside close relationships let's put
00:21:24.440 someone out in public among strangers how do you think about the variables that determine a person's
00:21:30.880 social experience yeah this one was a shocking one for me when i first started reading the literature
00:21:35.100 mostly because i'm not a very social person when it comes to strangers you know i'm the kind of person
00:21:39.820 that when i hop on a plane i put my huge headphones on in the hopes that no one will speak to me that's
00:21:44.480 what i used to do at least but yeah so there's so much work by folks like nick epley and liz dunn and
00:21:48.680 others that show that the simple like fleeting connections that we have with strangers can be really
00:21:54.940 powerful for our well-being and in this in this sense i really mean the sort of happy in your life
00:22:00.000 kind of well-being it really seems to bump up our positive emotions and so these are things like
00:22:04.640 you know the simple conversation you have with the barista at the coffee shop or you know chatting up
00:22:08.640 your uber driver while you're on your ride these kinds of simple social connections seem to bump up
00:22:14.200 our mood and the absence of them can seem to kind of decrease our mood in some interesting ways and so
00:22:20.320 what's striking about that intervention though is that people really don't think that's the case
00:22:25.320 this is work by nick epley he finds that people make incredibly strong predictions
00:22:28.960 that talking to strangers is going to be weird or awkward or just not very fun and what he finds
00:22:34.240 is that because of that misprediction people tend not to talk to strangers when they have the
00:22:38.060 opportunity to do so and that's true of introverts and extroverts i think the the bigger issue for
00:22:43.760 introverts is that that prediction in introverts is even stronger so if extroverts predict it'll be a
00:22:49.500 little awkward to you know talk with my uber driver for the whole ride introverts end up predicting
00:22:54.020 it's going to be actively awful to talk to my uber driver for the whole ride like it's going to make
00:22:58.220 be completely miserable but both extroverts and introverts nick fines actually get a big bump in
00:23:04.240 well-being from from having that conversation with the stranger and so and this actually is a theme
00:23:09.680 that's worth investigating further because i think this is part of a lot of the stuff we see in the
00:23:13.840 positive psychology work which is that there are these things that we can do to bump up our well-being
00:23:17.900 but by and large our theories about what we should do to bump up our well-being seem to be wrong which
00:23:22.980 is pretty frustrating because it means that like rational people aren't doing the things that they
00:23:27.340 should be doing to improve their happiness because they have misconceptions about the stuff that's going
00:23:31.180 to work right so so by theories you don't mean the scientific theories in the literature you mean
00:23:35.520 each person's personal idea about what they should do to become happier yeah not even in that rich of an
00:23:42.500 explicit sense right you know if i'm standing in line at starbucks to get a latte you know i have some
00:23:47.300 intuitions about what's going to make me happy i probably think the latte is going to you know bump
00:23:50.540 up my mood or bump up my productivity if i decide to talk to somebody it's because at least implicitly
00:23:55.280 i thought that would be a good idea it would kind of feel nice or feel good in that situation
00:23:59.100 and so we constantly have these very low-level automatic intuitions about the stuff that feels good
00:24:04.560 and that controls how we act in the world what's scary is that what the scientific theory suggests
00:24:09.740 that those intuitions are often wrong in other words we're systematically not doing the stuff
00:24:14.680 that could make us happiest yeah yeah so now how do you think about shyness in this context because
00:24:20.280 that obviously is the wall through which many people never push and it keeps them isolated in
00:24:28.600 a social circumstance where even if they accepted your thesis that they would be more or less guaranteed
00:24:36.200 to be happier if they could get to the other side of that wall it feels bad to even attempt it
00:24:43.420 yeah i mean i think i think what what the science suggests you should try if you're an introvert and
00:24:48.320 in that situation is just see if you could try it out like you know just you know take baby steps into
00:24:54.900 having conversations with strangers and then be mindful about how it feels and what nick's data
00:24:59.920 really suggests is that it's probably going to feel better than you expect the problem is that
00:25:04.240 there's a real startup cost to having those conversations because we have this strong intuition that it's going to
00:25:08.920 go really badly but if we can get over that startup cost the the benefits that we experience can be
00:25:13.920 really powerful and that's when i resonate with myself i mean i don't necessarily consider myself
00:25:18.240 an introvert but i'm definitely not the kind of person who just like typically strike up a conversation
00:25:22.780 with a complete stranger right actually my producer on my podcast ryan he's a journalist by nature and
00:25:27.520 every time we go out he's always talking to people and i'm like how do you do that you know it's not
00:25:31.840 yeah but but but the science suggests that i'm totally wrong i need to kind of bust through that
00:25:36.340 initial awkwardness and try it out and you get much more benefit than you'd expect so i'm like you in
00:25:42.280 that respect and it's always amazing to be with a friend who is the exact opposite and just see how
00:25:49.080 different their life is in situations like that i mean i i know people who can walk into a crowded
00:25:54.780 elevator and strike up a conversation with zero awkwardness and it is a kind of superpower
00:26:01.240 which i notice i entirely lack you just realize in those moments that there are people who are
00:26:07.580 walking through the world having a completely different experience because everywhere they're
00:26:11.620 going they're just talking to people and having a self-reinforcing and by and large entirely pleasant
00:26:18.940 encounter with the world whereas i don't know if you have any sense of the percentages here but
00:26:24.080 i would imagine increasingly so and we can talk about how our certain changes in our society
00:26:30.360 technologically have ramified this default setting of kind of eyes down isolation but
00:26:36.700 i would imagine most people at this point are walking through cities more or less ignoring everyone
00:26:43.840 most of the time at least to the limit of what's possible and the people who aren't doing that at all
00:26:50.180 are really they're living a very different parallel track of human experience yeah and and the data
00:26:57.500 suggests it's like a happier track yeah it's nice to see it's nice to know the researchers who study
00:27:02.180 this stuff because they often live it i remember going out to dinner with nick epley recently we were
00:27:06.820 in aspen colorado together and you know we you know went out to dinner and he will just strike up a
00:27:11.680 conversation with everyone you know the waitress is trying to come you know just take our order and
00:27:16.520 he'll end up chatting with her for like 15 minutes to the point that she's kind of having a good
00:27:19.660 time like oh sorry i have to go you know put your order in right or the person next to you and
00:27:23.600 you know it's it's foreign even though i know these data it's foreign to me it's not the thing
00:27:28.020 that i would normally do but i can resonate with them like wow that was so much more of a fun dinner
00:27:33.080 it went by so much faster because we were having all these interesting conversations and you know
00:27:37.280 the people around you are filled with interesting stories interesting ideas like we're social
00:27:41.900 primates we're going to get a lot out of that and so but but the message is that what we have to
00:27:46.740 do is to violate our intuitions and i think this is i mean this is so fundamental and i think it
00:27:51.360 doesn't get talked about enough like it really challenges our our rational approach to improving
00:27:56.140 our own well-being and to getting to eudaimonia if we have all these incorrect theories again
00:28:01.080 incorrect intuitions about the sorts of things that we need to do to be happy that means we could be
00:28:06.180 you know rationally following what our intuitions tell us but actively moving against our what would
00:28:12.460 be best for us in terms of our well-being which is so striking and that's why i find the science so
00:28:16.440 important it's because it doesn't totally cause you to update your intuitions you know i'm not
00:28:20.280 immediately a social person who's talking to the barista all the time but i can kind of put some
00:28:24.780 work in to overcome those and and it does make your life better if you can fight some of these bad
00:28:29.600 intuitions okay so we're having this conversation in the first week in april 2020 in a context where
00:28:38.260 for i would say at least a decade maybe a decade and a half we've witnessed a variety of social and
00:28:45.820 technological changes which again have made people in some ways less isolated but in a face-to-face
00:28:53.240 sense more isolated so the introduction of the smartphone is probably the biggest one you go into
00:28:59.060 public and people tend to close down any opportunity for spontaneous interaction with strangers because
00:29:08.600 they're virtually always looking at their phones whenever they get a chance and i guess on some level
00:29:14.720 they're socializing with somebody else often by doing that but it's not face-to-face and it's a
00:29:20.580 very different experience for social primates and i don't think i'm the only person to feel that we've
00:29:26.300 all been inducted into a psychological experiment to which more or less no one has really consented
00:29:32.540 and we're just rolling the dice with human psychology and seeing what comes of all this and now this is
00:29:39.680 in the context of this conversation especially ramified by the covet 19 pandemic where you have
00:29:46.520 people truly isolated for reasons of epidemiology and isolated under conditions of significant stress
00:29:56.200 if not health stress then economic stress so um i guess let's take both of those pieces how do you see
00:30:04.360 the trade-off between some of the technological advances we've made and they really have created
00:30:10.920 wealth and the ability to even have careers that would be unthinkable and the internet has been
00:30:16.460 you know obviously immensely useful and we're we're not going to get rid of it but it's easy to see how it
00:30:21.820 may have eroded face-to-face social connection for most people most of the time and then let's jump to the
00:30:30.440 current circumstance of the pandemic yeah i mean i worry a lot about what technology is doing to our
00:30:37.080 social connection you know if you if you get back to that barista in the coffee shop scenario you know
00:30:41.460 one of the reasons i don't talk to the barista is you know i'm not totally sure it'll be that fun
00:30:44.840 but also i don't even notice the barista right because i'm staring down at my phone and checking my
00:30:49.540 email or scrolling through my instagram feed or something and most of us are doing that and i think
00:30:55.340 you're right we we've put these devices in the pockets of you know six billion people around the
00:31:00.380 planet and we don't really know their cost there's there's now some good data coming out about the
00:31:05.680 the specific ways that that cell phones in general and technology in general might be affecting our
00:31:10.780 social relationships and they're all striking and really scary so liz dunn who's a professor at the
00:31:15.960 university of british columbia has been doing some of this work and she just does these really simple
00:31:19.580 experiments where she has say subjects sit together in a waiting room either with their cell phones or
00:31:24.400 without their cell phones and they're they're instructed just have the cell phone out so they're
00:31:27.720 not even necessarily using the cell phone it's just basically present and what she finds is that
00:31:32.120 the presence of a cell phone ends up decreasing the smiling between those subjects who are in the
00:31:37.180 waiting room by about 30 percent like just having it out and we see the same thing when we look at all
00:31:42.480 kinds of different social activities right she's another study where she has families going around like
00:31:47.880 say like a science museum together and she either lets the parents have their cell phones with them or not
00:31:52.940 and what she finds is way less enjoyment on the part of the parents in the science museum when they
00:31:58.120 have their cell phones out with their kids way less feeling like they bonded with their kids but also
00:32:02.020 the kids feel that way too right the kids are feeling less bonded with the parents too and again this is
00:32:07.040 just the mere presence of our cell phone and i think you know we often think you know when we when we
00:32:11.920 talk about technology is there's always like oh social media and facebook you know they're so evil and
00:32:16.100 they destroy connection like for me just as a basic scientist i actually worry more just about
00:32:21.500 what our attentional resources are doing when we're around cell phones right because in some ways like
00:32:27.640 our brain isn't stupid our brain knows what's on the other side of these devices and there's some pretty
00:32:31.960 good interesting you know panic inducing like exciting stuff on the other side of these things
00:32:36.420 and now those devices are competing with the basic social interactions we have like you know yeah i could
00:32:42.560 have a conversation with my husband over the dinner table but if i have my phone there i know that on
00:32:47.140 the other side of that phone is you know every political discussion that's happened you know every
00:32:50.740 cat video in the universe liz dunn i interviewed her for an upcoming season two episode of my podcast
00:32:56.320 and she had this wonderful analogy she said you know when you go to a dinner with your husband
00:32:59.860 imagine instead of bringing your phone you brought this big wheelbarrow and in the wheelbarrow is like
00:33:05.360 you know a printout of every book that's ever been written you know like printouts of every one of your
00:33:09.740 emails since 1992 like you're like a big big stacks of photo albums of all your family pictures
00:33:15.180 you know like every cat video a dvd of every cat video in the universe like you know every you know
00:33:20.380 museum like archive of every you know print that's ever been made in every art gallery all over the
00:33:25.340 world you know porn like like you know big dvd pile of porn he's like if you were sitting next to
00:33:30.340 that wheelbarrow during dinner you'd be distracted like you wouldn't want to talk to your husband
00:33:34.640 because you'd want to like flip through the dvds and see what cat videos were there right
00:33:37.940 and and what she says is like your brain knows that on the other end of that device is all that
00:33:42.640 stuff that wheelbarrow is there and even if you're paying attention to your conversation with your
00:33:46.960 husband at dinner there's a part of your mind that's distracted that you have to keep reeling
00:33:51.160 back from that big wheelbarrow of cat videos and we've put that distraction as i said in six billion
00:33:57.020 pockets around the world and we don't know what it's doing to our attentional resources we don't know
00:34:02.320 what it's doing to our social resources all we know is that it's a huge opportunity cost we haven't been
00:34:07.380 able to measure that cost yet but i think it's it's huge for our social relationships and also
00:34:12.020 you know given what we know about meditation and mind wandering i think it's huge for our well-being
00:34:16.180 too you know we're kind of constantly pulling our attention back from these devices in a way that
00:34:21.060 didn't exist 10 years ago we just didn't have that attentional cost 10 years ago and so so i find it
00:34:26.140 really scary but not for the reasons people typically think of i just think you know there's just an
00:34:30.860 attention suck that exists now that never existed before in human history and we have no idea what it's
00:34:35.980 doing to our minds and our relationships actually let's linger here before we get to the the pandemic
00:34:41.600 circumstance there's another variable here which is related to what technology is doing to us and how
00:34:49.600 we're essentially addicted to smartphones in particular and that's our our relationship to time
00:34:56.360 and the sense that we have to use it wisely that basically everything is an opportunity cost that
00:35:04.660 you know we're constantly triaging with respect to what we could be paying attention to and for many
00:35:10.900 of us certainly anyone who's a kind of knowledge worker there really is no boundary between the
00:35:18.440 moments where you could profitably get things done and you know any other moment in life wherever you
00:35:25.160 happen to be if i'm laurie santos in a starbucks in line there are many reasons not to look at the
00:35:32.560 barista but one is you can catch up on the emails that you know you're going to have to answer at some
00:35:38.500 point and if you answer a few now that's a few fewer you have to answer later in the day when you get
00:35:43.880 home there's just this fundamental erosion of the boundary between the imperative of getting stuff done
00:35:50.700 and all of these other moments in life and it's a creates a background level of stress for many of
00:35:58.080 us and there's something about our relationship to time that gets changed there i don't know if you
00:36:04.700 have any thoughts on that yeah i think that's really important you know so one of the other
00:36:09.100 things we know is super important for well-being is is our perception of time there's some lovely work
00:36:14.380 coming out of ashley willens lab at harvard business school focused on this concept of time
00:36:18.220 affluence right which is the subjective feeling that you have a lot of free time the opposite is
00:36:22.960 time famine where you feel kind of famished for time hungry for time and ashley's work has been
00:36:27.780 showing that physiologically time famine works a lot like hunger famine where you're kind of triaging
00:36:32.500 it sort of pumps up your stress hormones and so on ashley's also shown that so what's odd about
00:36:37.100 what we what's odd about our sense of time right now is that we assume we have less free time but
00:36:42.260 actually if you look at people's calendars they actually have more free time all told which is kind of
00:36:47.760 surprising we feel like we're so time famished right now the problem is that our time right now
00:36:51.840 is broken up into into what she's called time confetti so we have free time but it's in these
00:36:56.700 like tiny snippets and i think that the the form of those tiny snippets is exactly has the features
00:37:02.260 of exactly what you're saying which is that it's it's hard to use those in a way that promotes our
00:37:07.000 well-being you know it's hard to like you know have a deep conversation with our spouse when we have
00:37:10.660 like five minutes here and there like it's like well let me just get a few emails off the cuff you
00:37:14.660 know if i have this little bit of free time and so even when we have these moments of free time
00:37:18.940 confetti we end up using them kind of for work stuff or or for stuff that's really fast right you
00:37:25.220 know i sometimes when i'm doing kind of exactly what you're saying i'm in line i could get a few
00:37:29.120 emails down but that's anxiety provoking you know i'll just do a quick panic scroll through my social
00:37:33.360 media feed or you know like like look up that news article and so on and so we end up because the time
00:37:38.820 is so cut up and in these tiny bits we end up using it for what feels like the easiest thing
00:37:44.320 right things that have a little bit of a startup cost or things that you know take a little bit
00:37:48.780 of time to get going we tend not to prioritize and that means we're not prioritizing a lot of deep
00:37:54.440 social connection with people because you know that has this kind of startup cost and takes a little
00:37:58.680 time it often means we're not even prioritizing like good leisure either you know the time confetti
00:38:04.420 means we don't want to you know learn a new instrument or learn a new language or even you
00:38:08.580 know dive into like a deep novel you know all those existential novels you were talking about before
00:38:12.560 like i'm not you know picking up a good like you know such novel because it feels too much like i'm
00:38:17.740 just going to scroll through the new york times or i'm just going to like pick up my twitter feed
00:38:20.500 like we kind of only have time for you know like a few characters because the time is so broken up
00:38:26.160 and so i think i think this time confetti has lots of consequences for our happiness and i think you're
00:38:31.940 right that the fact that these technologies are breaking up our time in these ways is having a
00:38:37.820 negative effect but but but the technologies also have a negative effect on time in a completely
00:38:42.320 different way which is that unlike the other important things in our life be it you know social
00:38:47.000 relationships or sleep those things don't nag us as much as our devices do right you know my husband
00:38:52.340 doesn't have a notification ding that like comes up you know in my window when i'm checking my email too
00:38:57.320 long you know but my email does when i'm talking to my husband right and so i think you know the the
00:39:02.560 fact of the matter is is that most parts of our technology most apps in these things you know get
00:39:07.940 get revenue and get money from having eyeballs on them and so you know your iphone wants to kind of
00:39:13.140 remind you to be using your iphone and all the apps on your iphone want to be reminding you to use
00:39:17.420 those apps and that means that they've kind of start bugging you in a way that the other important
00:39:22.960 things in life don't and it's really hard to ignore those things you know because they're built
00:39:27.100 on the latest neuroscience of what grabs your attention of what kind of gives you a little
00:39:31.000 dopamine hit so you feel like it'll be rewarding to jump on that phone and real life doesn't do that
00:39:36.360 you know real life doesn't have teams of designers trying to like mess with our attention and mess with
00:39:40.800 our dopamine and that's problematic because it means that the technology kind of grabs our attention
00:39:46.300 easier and when you add to that the normal time confetti that all of us have where we're kind of
00:39:50.380 just going to go with the easy thing you know it's kind of a recipe for not prioritizing the right
00:39:54.440 stuff in our lives right yeah the technology has changed the way we initiate social contact even with
00:40:01.260 people who are who are our closest friends it used to be that you would just pick up the phone and call
00:40:07.480 someone and that wasn't a surprising intrusion into their solitude which it is now i mean i feel like a
00:40:16.500 cold call even from someone i'm close to with a few exceptions my wife is an exception and maybe there's
00:40:24.060 a couple of other people in my life who i still expect a call from but virtually every other call
00:40:29.100 the default now is to set it up by email or by text and just a cold call is almost analogous to
00:40:36.920 20 years ago how it would have felt if someone just showed up at your house unannounced and rang the
00:40:42.500 doorbell and you know i don't know if you have noticed this in your life but there's been a
00:40:47.240 migration from email to text now and you know it's very short form punctate communication i mean now
00:40:55.480 you know text rather often is a surrogate for maintaining the relationship in the old way which
00:41:01.760 is actually seeing or speaking with each other yeah and we know that i mean obviously the it means
00:41:07.260 those are shorter communications right because you're not kind of having long lingering conversations
00:41:11.120 but but it's also missing all the stuff that we're built as primates to pay attention to
00:41:15.440 right you don't get the right emotion through text as you do through changes to my vocal intonation or
00:41:21.380 subtle changes to my facial expression we haven't as a species gotten good at using text to do that stuff
00:41:27.060 yet long text right you know if you read you know again a fantastic novel you can see pathos in there
00:41:32.180 you can see the emotion but you don't really get that you know in a short text you know about dinner and
00:41:36.640 when dinner is ready kind of thing and so i think we're we're we don't realize what we're missing out
00:41:41.540 on in those interactions yeah and i and i think you know again it's just crazy that we've had this
00:41:49.440 experiment on human psychology and put you know basically changed around six billion social
00:41:54.240 relationships without people's permission and not knowing really how it's going to have long-term
00:41:58.940 effects but i think we're starting to see the long-term effects i mean this is the mental health crisis
00:42:02.740 that we've been seeing exploding in all generations but particularly in young people who've for the most
00:42:08.600 part only ever known these forms of communication you know this is the explosion of loneliness that
00:42:13.020 we've seen you know loneliness has been increasing by double digit numbers in the last decade and you
00:42:18.520 know in some ways it's ironic because these technologies were supposed to be linking us up
00:42:22.280 but in practice they could be they could be failing to allow us to connect in the ways that our primate
00:42:28.760 minds are used to connecting and that can have all kinds of consequences we don't realize
00:42:32.320 so how have you been thinking about the covet 19 experience we're all having in really for most of us
00:42:41.200 in genuine isolation albeit in many cases with our families and many of us are experiencing a silver
00:42:48.420 lining there where we're having more enforced quality time with with our families but it is a
00:42:54.540 generally speaking a surreal upheaval in it's a psychological experiment of a different order
00:43:00.320 now and we've all been inducted into it yeah i think it's i mean i mean first of all it's just
00:43:06.580 surreal and crazy you know this is what it must have felt like to live through other major natural
00:43:11.340 disasters for our species you know i feel like we're dinosaurs watching the meteor hit in some ways but
00:43:15.600 i mean i think the the biggest upheaval as you said is is in our social relationships in our social
00:43:20.500 lives and i think you know if you look at what happens when people are going through a tough stressful
00:43:25.620 time what our species does is we try to hook up with other people like we try to hang out with our
00:43:31.500 friends you know we go to our mom's house and get a hug if that's possible like we we just try to
00:43:35.820 connect as much as we possibly can and in terms of our physical health that's impossible right now
00:43:41.780 like to flatten the curve we just can't do that there's there's an additional feature that's bad for
00:43:46.280 social relationships too which is that if you think about what the threat is in the covet 19 crisis
00:43:50.800 it's other people you know it's that guy that touched my doorknob before i walked out of my
00:43:55.300 house you know it's the person who's panic buying the toilet paper that i need like in addition to
00:43:59.920 not being able to connect with people which is our natural response during a crisis other people are
00:44:05.080 part of the crisis you know they're kind of making the crisis worse and i think those two things
00:44:08.900 together are you know making this an incredibly challenging time is making an otherwise incredibly
00:44:13.900 challenging time even more challenging the good news though is i think this is the time when we can
00:44:18.960 start to harness some of those technologies for social connection in even better ways and i what
00:44:25.760 i mean by that is that it's not just a matter of like hey go on zoom and you know talk to some friend
00:44:30.700 over zoom it's trying to find ways to use these technologies to get the informal social connections
00:44:36.220 that we're missing out on so much you know it's you know many of us as you said you know some people
00:44:40.440 are living in isolation and i think for them you know it's a completely different matter but you know
00:44:44.140 some of us have family members and so on but you know we're missing the chat with our co-workers
00:44:48.420 at the water cooler you know we're missing that quick conversation with the barista at the coffee
00:44:52.180 shop or just the smiles that we give to people when we walk down the street you know those have
00:44:56.300 gone away a little bit over time but they're still there and i think a lot of us are are facing the
00:45:01.440 need the craving that we're getting from not having that stuff there's a new really cool paper by
00:45:06.280 rebecca sachs who's a neuroscientist at mit who'd actually started this work a long time ago but it just
00:45:11.560 got published during covid showing that if you put people in social isolation the the areas of their
00:45:17.520 brain that would normally show craving for things like food and so on start craving social connection
00:45:22.500 so basically the kind of hunger craving that we get for say sugary foods when we stop eating those
00:45:27.220 that's the kind of thing that we get for social connection after a really short period of social
00:45:31.100 isolation and so i think a lot of us are going to be going through that right now but again the good
00:45:35.500 news is that there are these mechanisms of connecting with other people i think we just have to use them
00:45:40.300 to to replicate the informal social social connections too like those are the ones we kind of need
00:45:45.560 like so so for example as i made a in a made a zoom meeting to hang out with a friend of mine
00:45:50.740 who's in new york while i was just chopping vegetables you know for dinner and i was like hey
00:45:54.520 you know just like be there while i'm chopping vegetables and her you know face was on the screen and
00:45:58.480 i'm talking vegetables and we're chatting but but in that you have a couple things i can see her
00:46:02.320 facial expressions you know she can hear me laughing we can we can hear each other's intonation
00:46:06.540 in our voices i can see her in real time you know it's not face to face but it's it's pretty good
00:46:13.600 for what our primate beings are sucking up or at least it's much better than scrolling an instagram
00:46:17.620 feed or looking at a text thread we just have to kind of build that in but but the problem you know
00:46:22.820 as we talked about is right we have to get over that thing that we have kind of built in through
00:46:26.840 these technologies of like i gotta call somebody and that feels awkward and it's kind of the startup
00:46:31.040 cost but i think you know remarkably our norms changed really fast i mean even just for me personally
00:46:36.340 it felt you know those first zoom calls are like we're gonna play trivia over zoom friends you
00:46:40.540 know felt a little like oh this is kind of weird but you know within three weeks of doing it that's
00:46:44.700 just kind of how we connect now like it it becomes normal surprisingly quickly to use these technologies
00:46:50.200 in these informal social ways i want to recall something you just said about being other oriented
00:46:56.900 and the payoffs of that i mean this is a very buddhist concept if you want to be happier help somebody
00:47:06.000 else essentially the the algorithm or even just intend to help somebody else think positively about
00:47:13.280 somebody else's well-being and you'll find you're gladdening your own mind how do you think about that
00:47:19.780 and the larger framework in which people pursue that so you know we can think about ethics and having
00:47:28.020 some kind of actual conscious conception of the type of person one wants to be the kinds of virtues
00:47:34.060 one wants to actually live out in one's life this is where so-called self-sacrifice becomes the wiser
00:47:41.580 form of selfishness if you really just want to be happy if that's your goal one fairly wide doorway
00:47:49.480 into that is to be very rigorous about using your energy in a consciously pro-social way to improve
00:47:58.840 the lives of others so what do we know about all of that yeah well what we know is i mean the science
00:48:04.380 suggests that you know as usual the buddhists were right you know all these ancient traditions wound up
00:48:08.340 being confirmed by modern social science and neuroscience but yeah i mean the the happiest
00:48:12.860 folks tend to be on average the folks that give more to charity even equated for income the happiest folks
00:48:18.600 tend to be the ones that on average volunteer more of their time and are just kind of you know as you
00:48:23.280 said kind of ethically oriented to kind of thinking about other people first but i think this is
00:48:27.560 another spot where our intuitions get it all wrong you know if you look at you know any like self-help
00:48:32.920 magazine or any article these days especially during covet 19 it's all about self-help you know
00:48:38.380 self-help you know self-care treat yourself you know this is parks and rec slogan that we need to be
00:48:43.260 treating ourselves i think we think that when push comes to shove the way to get out of a stressful
00:48:47.920 situation is to become more inward oriented like focus on what we ourselves think we need
00:48:53.160 hedonistically or in terms of our you know like meaning and life and leisure and stuff like that
00:48:59.020 and the science suggests that that again is an intuition that's just incredibly wrong you know
00:49:03.960 there's some work by liz dunn and her colleagues and that shows that spending money on yourself
00:49:08.820 actually makes you less happy than spending money on other people you know she does these lovely
00:49:13.500 studies where she just walks up to somebody on a street and hands them money and tells the subjects how
00:49:17.300 to spend it and so some half of the subjects are told spend the money on other people by the end of the
00:49:21.520 day and and some of them are told spend the money on yourself by the end of the day and what she
00:49:25.600 finds is that the people who spend money on other people at the end of the day and even later on like
00:49:30.080 at the end of the week are happier self-reported are happier than those who spent the money on
00:49:34.600 themselves and i think that you know and she also like nick epley does work showing that that's not
00:49:39.580 people's intuitions you know she asks a different group of subjects which of these conditions would
00:49:43.240 make you happier and the subjects are in pretty strong agreement that they want the money for
00:49:47.160 themselves that's the kind of thing that would make them happier and so you know i think it's one of
00:49:50.780 these things that like ethically and in terms of our religious commitments those of us who are
00:49:54.680 religious like people kind of get that you're supposed to do nice stuff for others but but but
00:50:00.040 often people think about that in a like well that's to be a good person it doesn't necessarily make me in
00:50:05.380 the moment happier to do something nice for somebody else you know it's kind of a sacrifice right
00:50:09.200 but in practice what the science suggests is that that's wrong like if i'm having a really bad day at
00:50:14.440 work i shouldn't go off and buy myself a manicure i should just like get a gift card to give one of my
00:50:18.680 co-workers a manicure and that intuition feels just wrong to me you know maybe it's the right
00:50:23.280 thing to do or a noble thing to do or you know a very ethical thing to do like philosophers would
00:50:27.580 be really proud of me but i don't think that like laurie's own dopamine system is going to respond
00:50:31.820 better to gifting that manicure than getting it myself but that's actually what the data suggest
00:50:36.640 yeah this is where mindfulness can be very helpful because you can notice the hedonic bump when you do that
00:50:44.060 sort of thing and it can become more and more vivid and also you can notice the ways in which
00:50:49.320 giving what in real terms is even more i mean just like you're writing a check to an organization
00:50:55.520 there are ways to do that where you get very little hedonic reinforcement and there are ways to do that
00:51:03.100 where you get much more and it's it's interesting the variables there but it'd be great if there were a
00:51:09.260 truly linear connection between doing good things in the world and moment-to-moment gratification but
00:51:16.240 there's definitely a connection but it's just it requires some intelligent steering of your own
00:51:22.560 attention to extract the reward that is there to be extracted this circumstance of being in economic
00:51:30.740 lockdown offers some unique opportunities to experience this so you know i've noticed that anyone who
00:51:39.000 is fairly well off in this situation who's you know hasn't experienced an implosion economically
00:51:47.180 and who can continue working really the low-hanging fruit here ethically is to continue to support the
00:51:56.260 people in one's life who you know are just being cratered by this change in the economy so take somebody
00:52:04.580 in a service role the first person to come to mind for me was the woman who cuts my hair i get a haircut
00:52:11.840 you know i don't know once every six weeks or so and i had to know that her business was more or less
00:52:18.700 going to zero under these conditions so very early on i had the thought well i'll just buy imaginary
00:52:25.040 haircuts there's no reason why you know she should suffer the fact that i can't physically get those
00:52:30.700 haircuts and just doing that and a few things like that i mean it wasn't a list of a hundred people
00:52:35.620 like that in my life but taking care of those people when i was truly sacrificing nothing to do it
00:52:42.620 that's some of the most pleasant experience i've had all month just being able to do that and
00:52:48.600 and i would just argue it's a good thing to do it's good for you and it's good for the world
00:52:53.320 yeah totally i mean i want to pick up on two points here one is just i think you've you've kind of
00:52:58.540 completely hit the nail on the head right there i think this is covet 19 is a time where we really
00:53:03.900 feel like we don't have that much agency right i mean the maximally frustrating thing is do you want
00:53:08.220 to help just stay home and don't do anything like just stay home like don't do anything and you know
00:53:12.860 humans don't like that we like to be you know causally effective in the world and i think one way
00:53:17.440 to be causally effective is just to be helping financially if again you're in the privileged position
00:53:22.920 to do that all the people you would have normally helped financially anyway and in some ways
00:53:27.800 as you said there's no cost to it like that money was already spent and i think that's an
00:53:33.080 important framing for this time is that many of us are getting financial windfalls that we're not
00:53:38.420 paying attention to you know i'm not spending you know four bucks on a latte every morning which was
00:53:43.180 my normal practice you know some of us are not paying you know the subway fare or the gas fare for
00:53:48.560 our commutes these are all tiny windfalls that lots of us are getting in so many domains during
00:53:53.480 covet 19 but we can pay those windfalls back to the people that need it right now and even folks
00:53:58.860 who are in not great financial positions because probably a lot of your listeners aren't in the same
00:54:03.280 privileged position that you are you know to know that they still have a job some of them you are
00:54:07.240 working less hours or maybe even have lost their jobs and so on even those folks have a different
00:54:12.240 windfall they have a temporal windfall you know they have time that they might not have had before
00:54:16.780 and again the best use of time in terms of your well-being is time spent on other people you know
00:54:22.720 so you can be making those calls to advocate for say more ppe for health care workers you know you
00:54:27.880 can make a call to an elderly neighbor to kind of check on how they are and those kinds of ways of
00:54:33.520 spending our money and our time during this crisis can have a huge impact on our well-being
00:54:37.920 personally but then also they're just like good for the world because we're like doing good stuff to
00:54:42.440 like protect the economy and protect the vulnerable folks during this crisis but but i also
00:54:46.500 want to pick up on a second thing which is this idea you know you mentioned that to notice the
00:54:50.600 effect that your your good actions have on your own psyche you kind of have to be a little bit
00:54:55.780 mindful and i think this is really powerful this is something that i think neuroscience is just
00:55:00.460 beginning to understand which is how we can use mindfulness to to hack these bad intuitions that we
00:55:06.840 have about stuff you know throughout this conversation i've been saying you know we should be more
00:55:10.000 social but we don't realize that and therefore we don't do it we should be nicer to other people
00:55:13.540 be more focused on other people but we don't notice it so we don't realize we should do it
00:55:17.920 mindfulness the research is starting to suggest is one way to hack those things so that you can
00:55:23.200 start to notice hang on when i actually do this it feels nice and that while it doesn't immediately
00:55:28.660 change your intuitions it can kind of change your reinforcement structure such that you start to
00:55:33.740 realize what these things really look like and this comes from some lovely work coming out of
00:55:38.120 heady cober's lab she's a neuroscientist at yale who uses mindfulness techniques to to do all kinds
00:55:43.720 of different therapeutic things including working with addicts on their craving and so on and and it's
00:55:48.800 a powerful technique because even even in domains like you know an addict who has craving for say
00:55:53.760 nicotine or heroin or something like that the act of noticing what it's like afterwards can update
00:55:59.240 these circuits that are getting the getting the wanting wrong you know one of the one of the worst
00:56:04.480 things about the mind is like the most one of the most shocking things i ever read in my early
00:56:08.480 psychology training was that there's this interesting disconnect in the brain between
00:56:13.100 circuits that are involved in wanting and sort of craving and circuits that are involved in liking
00:56:18.460 and so the circuit that tells your your body hey go out and crave this thing go get it no matter
00:56:23.720 what cost work work work really hard to get it that's completely different from the circuit that's
00:56:27.680 actually going to like the thing once you get it and you can see these crazy dissociations where
00:56:32.320 like in the case of addiction where we can have incredible craving for something you know work
00:56:37.240 really hard to get it you know take the heroin addict who's addicted to heroin but then when you
00:56:41.240 finally get that reward you don't actually like it that much it's actually not even that rewarding
00:56:45.280 you know the heroin to an addicted heroin addict is just bringing you to baseline it's not even that
00:56:49.540 good anymore yeah and this this i feel like you know is true in addictions but it's so true in so many
00:56:54.780 aspects of my life before i kind of started practicing meditation and mindfulness where it's like there's all
00:56:59.640 these things that my body wants me to go after all the time that i think is going to be really great
00:57:04.420 because my craving's super high for it but then when i get it i'm kind of like if you actually
00:57:08.100 notice you're like well that wasn't that good like that kind of sucked or like that didn't make me feel
00:57:13.360 what i thought it was going to make me feel and then there's stuff like we're talking about about
00:57:16.960 doing nice things for others where at least for me i don't necessarily have the craving for it you
00:57:21.820 know as i said on a bad day i'm not thinking let me give a gift card you know for the manicure to my
00:57:26.040 co-worker i'm thinking let me get the manicure myself but then actually if you're mindful and
00:57:30.600 you pay attention afterwards you could notice even though the craving the wanting wasn't that high
00:57:34.980 the liking is pretty good and it can cause you to start shifting your behaviors and so so heady's
00:57:39.880 starting to do some real work on the actual neuroscience of this like you know what is it
00:57:44.180 about this act of mindfully noticing that can then feed back on your behaviors so you're kind of
00:57:48.960 updating what desires you really do want to have over time yeah yeah that's fascinating
00:57:54.260 mindfulness also can show you that desire doesn't have to be gratified to disappear right if you just
00:58:02.700 become interested in desire itself as a an object of consciousness right and you just become committed
00:58:08.580 to witnessing it arise and persist for a time and then pass away it will in fact pass away and in
00:58:16.440 many cases i mean obviously you can resurrect it again by focusing on on the wanted object yet again but
00:58:23.180 you can sensitize yourself to this full time course of desires arising and subsiding and realize that
00:58:30.700 there's nothing you have to do about it it's almost like the abandoned shopping cart of the mind
00:58:37.360 we've all had this experience that you go to zappos or whatever and you pick out a pair of shoes but then
00:58:42.020 you think better of it and then those shoes follow you around for the rest of your life online but
00:58:47.680 you can abandon the shopping cart and it really can just disappear then one wonders okay well then
00:58:55.200 what is the significance of gratifying any specific desire and then on the other side as you say you can
00:59:03.140 become more mindful of what it's like to gratify a desire and
00:59:08.700 if you'd like to continue listening to this podcast you'll need to subscribe at samharris.org
00:59:20.120 you'll get access to all full-length episodes of the making sense podcast and to other subscriber
00:59:25.180 only content including bonus episodes and amas and the conversations i've been having on the waking
00:59:30.780 up app the making sense podcast is ad-free and relies entirely on listener support and you can
00:59:36.900 subscribe now at samharris.org