Making Sense - Sam Harris - November 10, 2021


#267 — The Kingdom of Sleep


Episode Stats

Length

49 minutes

Words per Minute

156.33958

Word Count

7,762

Sentence Count

366

Misogynist Sentences

2


Summary

Matthew Walker is a professor of neuroscience and psychology at UC Berkeley and the Director of its Sleep and Neuroimaging Lab. He s also a former professor of psychiatry at Harvard University and has published over 100 scientific studies and has appeared on 60 Minutes, Nova, BBC News, and many other outlets. His first book, Why We Sleep, has been an international bestseller, and he also hosts his own podcast, The Matt Walker Podcast. In this episode, we discuss sleep and consciousness, the stages of sleep, the evolutionary origins of sleep and the generally doomed attempt to reduce one s need for sleep. We discuss sleep hygiene, caffeine and alcohol, sleep efficiency, bedtime restriction, napping, and finally, sleep tracking. And as we ll hear, each of us is associated with the company Aura, which makes a sleep tracking ring. And you ll hear about the connection between my own Aura Ring and Orthosomnia, which is a remarkable device I may have a love-hate relationship with. In any case, make of that what you will, and I hope you find this conversation useful as it runs nearly four hours. And we ll get into all of that in this episode of the Making Sense Podcast, hosted by Sam Harris. Thanks for joining me, it s a delight and a privilege to be speaking with you! Sam Harris - The Making Sense Podcatcher and in this podcast, by . by Matt Walker is a writer, researcher, and podcast host, and is a regular contributor to The New York Times, The New Yorker, The Huffington Post, and The Atlantic, and the New York Review, and so much more. Please consider becoming a supporter of what we re doing here. by becoming one of us, becoming a friend of us on the making sense podcast, by becoming a member of The Making sense Podcast on Insta- and we re making sense of it. , and we ll talk about it on social media, too, too of it, too , so you can help us make it so we do it on the Making sense of that, too we do that, and we get it like that, we can do it, we really do it all that we are making it so good, we like it, and that s not just that, so we know it, and they do it so much of it it s that, really are that


Transcript

00:00:00.000 Welcome to the Making Sense Podcast. This is Sam Harris. Just a note to say that if
00:00:12.180 you're hearing this, you are not currently on our subscriber feed and will only be hearing
00:00:16.320 the first part of this conversation. In order to access full episodes of the Making Sense
00:00:20.820 Podcast, you'll need to subscribe at samharris.org. There you'll find our private RSS feed to
00:00:26.420 add to your favorite podcatcher, along with other subscriber-only content. We don't run ads on the
00:00:31.580 podcast, and therefore it's made possible entirely through the support of our subscribers. So if you
00:00:36.360 enjoy what we're doing here, please consider becoming one. Today I'm speaking with Matthew
00:00:48.500 Walker. Matt is a professor of neuroscience and psychology at UC Berkeley and the director of
00:00:54.580 its Sleep and Neuroimaging Lab, and he's also a former professor of psychiatry at Harvard University.
00:01:00.680 He has published over 100 scientific studies and has appeared on 60 Minutes, Nova, BBC News,
00:01:08.080 and many other outlets. His first book, Why We Sleep, has been an international bestseller,
00:01:13.800 and he also hosts his own podcast, the Matt Walker Podcast. I've been wanting to speak to Matt for
00:01:20.980 quite some time because, as you'll hear, I've been increasingly worried about the quality of my own
00:01:25.520 sleep. I'm late to the party here, but now I'm convinced of the importance of sleeping well most
00:01:33.200 nights. And Matt and I get into all the details here about the nature and importance of sleep.
00:01:40.080 We discuss sleep and consciousness, the stages of sleep, sleep regularity, light and temperature,
00:01:47.820 the evolutionary origins of sleep, the generally doomed attempt to reduce one's need for sleep,
00:01:55.920 the connection between deficiencies in sleep and all-cause mortality, Alzheimer's disease, diabetes,
00:02:04.540 obesity, and heart disease, the role that sleep plays in learning and memory and mental health,
00:02:10.400 heart rate variability, REM sleep behavior disorder, and various parasomnias. We discuss lucid dreaming,
00:02:19.680 dreams as a kind of therapy, the connection between meditation and sleep, the various forms of insomnia,
00:02:27.580 and there are practical tips for what to do about them strewn throughout our conversation.
00:02:31.980 We discuss sleep hygiene, caffeine and alcohol, sleep efficiency, bedtime restriction,
00:02:40.920 napping, and finally sleep tracking. And as we'll hear on that final topic of sleep tracking,
00:02:48.700 Matt and I discover that each of us is associated with the company Aura that makes a sleep tracking ring.
00:02:54.920 I am a minor investor in the company, and Matt is its scientific advisor. Neither of us knew about
00:03:01.220 the connection before we started talking, and you'll hear I have a bit of a love-hate relationship
00:03:06.040 with my own Aura ring. It is a remarkable device, but I may have what Matt calls orthosomnia,
00:03:12.940 which is an overabundance of concern about my sleep data. In any case, make of that what you will,
00:03:19.160 and I hope you find this conversation useful as it runs nearly four hours. And now I bring you Matthew
00:03:26.340 Walker. I am here with Matthew Walker. Matt, thanks for joining me. It's a delight and a privilege to be
00:03:39.560 speaking with you, Sam. Thanks for having me. So you've written a book, Why We Sleep, that seems to
00:03:45.860 have gotten into the hands, if not the brains of more or less everyone. And now you have your own
00:03:53.040 podcast, the Matt Walker podcast. And you have been on many, many podcasts that I've noticed talking
00:04:01.040 about the science of sleep and seemingly almost single-handedly making people newly aware of the
00:04:13.060 importance of sleep in their lives, both from the side of, you know, physical health and mental health,
00:04:20.140 emotional regulation, really just across the board when you're talking about human well-being.
00:04:25.960 The difference between good and bad sleep seems paramount. And I must say, I have really neglected
00:04:33.460 sleep as a variable for most of my life. In fact, I think I was early in life toyed with the, you know,
00:04:43.140 fairly crazy ideal of limiting sleep so as to boost productivity. And we'll get into all of that. But
00:04:50.880 before we dive into the specific chapters of our conversation here, perhaps you can introduce
00:04:56.700 yourself, your background intellectually and academically, and just tell us how you came to
00:05:03.240 focus on sleep.
00:05:06.440 I wish I could take the compliment of bringing sleep back onto the public awareness map. I stand on the
00:05:13.580 shoulders of many of my colleagues and they are astronomically wonderful. So I try to do my part.
00:05:19.640 In terms of my background, I am a professor of neuroscience and psychology at the University
00:05:26.100 of California, Berkeley in America. And I've really tried to dedicate myself to understanding the
00:05:35.420 question of why we sleep for the past 20 years. I think like most people, I am an accidental sleep
00:05:42.800 researcher. I often think, you know, when kids are young and the teacher says, tell me what
00:05:49.620 you would like to be when you grow up. No one's shooting their hand up in the classroom and
00:05:54.320 saying, I desperately want to be a sleep researcher.
00:05:58.020 Yeah. And I can attest that when I started my neuroscience PhD, someone from a sleep lab,
00:06:04.620 I forget who, tried to recruit me to their lab. And I thought, why would I want to study sleep?
00:06:11.800 I had no interest at that point. And, you know, now I feel some chagrin over that dismissal because
00:06:19.940 it is increasingly fascinating. And as I said, consequential.
00:06:24.280 And in some ways, I, you know, I don't blame you. Maybe at the time, certainly even 20 years ago,
00:06:28.860 one could argue it's almost academic suicide to suggest that you want to become a sleep researcher
00:06:34.600 and not necessarily truthful, but some would argue that it was almost a charlatan science
00:06:41.480 to begin with. And of course it is. It's the most bizarre, strange, illogical, irrational,
00:06:48.620 from an evolutionary perspective, idiotic thing that an organism can do. And you're going to
00:06:55.120 leverage an entire academic career on that platform. Good luck and good night would be the,
00:07:00.720 I think, the tagline. But I was studying for my PhD, people with different forms of dementia.
00:07:09.280 And I was using brainwave patterns to try and differentially diagnose them very early on in
00:07:14.060 the course of dementia. And I was failing miserably, couldn't get any good results.
00:07:19.480 And one weekend, I had this little igloo of journals that I would retreat to, which tells you
00:07:25.000 everything about my social life. And I started to learn that some of those dementias
00:07:30.540 would eat away at sleep centers and other forms of the dementias would not, because there are many
00:07:35.820 different forms of dementia. So I realized I was measuring my patients at the wrong time,
00:07:39.960 which was when they were awake, and I should be measuring them when they were asleep.
00:07:44.100 I started doing that. I got some fantastic results. And at that point, I started to ask the question,
00:07:51.520 I wonder if these sleep disruptions and impairments are not a consequence of the dementia.
00:07:58.780 They're not a symptom of the dementia. Maybe they are a cause of the dementia. But I realized 20 years
00:08:05.320 ago, no one could answer a very fundamental question, which was, why do we sleep? And I think
00:08:10.860 the crass answer at that time was that we sleep to cure sleepiness, which is the fatuous equivalent of
00:08:18.660 saying, I eat to cure hunger. It tells you nothing about the unique benefits.
00:08:23.240 But then I started to explore this thing called sleep, and I fell absolutely in love with it. And to
00:08:33.220 this day, 20 years on, I still think it is the most beguiling thing in science. It is a love affair that's
00:08:39.840 not left me for all of those decades. And I remain an amorous partner to its wonderful gifts, both
00:08:48.520 nightly as a practice and also from an intellectual and academic and research perspective. Does that
00:08:55.380 give some background?
00:08:56.000 Yeah, yeah. Well, if I can follow your romantic analogy here, sleep is a fairly coy mistress for
00:09:04.300 many of us. And this, you know, speaking personally, this has always been not even on the back burner for
00:09:13.780 me as a problem to solve in my life. I just, I've accustomed myself to sleeping badly and just accepting
00:09:23.000 on some level that I sleep badly. And so, encountering your work is fairly arresting to someone in my
00:09:31.440 condition, because the stakes, as we will elucidate here, are incredibly high, given the connection between
00:09:39.500 sleep and health. So, I wanted to, at the outset, address the component of worry here, worry about
00:09:48.720 sleep, because many people listening to us will also recognize in themselves that their sleep is
00:09:55.340 far from ideal. And to add a layer of worry to that is obviously counterproductive when the goal is to
00:10:05.560 make it easier to sleep soundly and on some better schedule in general. So, can you address this effect
00:10:14.420 that our conversation is likely to have, especially when we're talking about possible links between
00:10:20.900 poor sleep and dementia and, you know, all the rest? It's just, it's very easy to begin to treat
00:10:28.440 this as some kind of medical emergency in the offing. What do you have to say as a, by way of
00:10:35.220 guidance or caution on that point?
00:10:37.660 In some ways, it's a rock and a hard place that I've found myself in. And this is something that
00:10:43.920 I've learned since publishing the book. And I think it's something that I've corrected in my
00:10:48.600 communication to the public. As I was writing the book at the time, at least within the public sphere,
00:10:54.300 as you mentioned, sleep was the neglected step system in the health conversation of today. And it
00:11:00.380 was that way. And I was so familiar, as all of my colleagues were, with the disease and the sickness
00:11:07.500 and the suffering that was happening because of this sleep deficiency that was so pernicious throughout
00:11:14.120 most first world nations, that I wanted to try to, no pun intended for either this podcast or the
00:11:24.060 topic, but sort of wake people up to the fact of the importance of sleep. And I think that in my
00:11:30.300 communications and maybe even in segments of the book, I was perhaps heavy handed. And I had neglected
00:11:38.580 to recognize the concern for the sleep anxious and those who are having sleep difficulty. And I've
00:11:46.560 since become so much more sensitive to that. And I can't deny the science. I can't not tell you
00:11:56.920 about the links between insufficient sleep and Alzheimer's disease, obesity, diabetes, cardiovascular
00:12:04.580 disease, depression, anxiety, even suicide, some forms of cancer. But I also don't want people to
00:12:12.520 become overly anxious. But how do you do that? How do you find that sweet spot? And so for me, it's
00:12:20.540 been a real lesson and a lesson also because I am no poster child for sleep. I have had my battles and
00:12:27.560 I did not mention them in the book. And I think I should have. I'm being personally open. I'm a very
00:12:33.200 private person. I've had at least three bouts of insomnia during my lifetime and they were vicious.
00:12:39.740 And just because you know a little about sleep doesn't mean as though you are immune to its
00:12:45.860 vagaries. It is a mistress that can be very fickle. So I think for this podcast, it's important to keep
00:12:54.620 in mind two things. First, everyone has a bad night of sleep. And if you're there at night struggling to fall
00:13:03.480 asleep, don't worry. Even with all of the facts and the science that we will discuss, it's not the worst
00:13:11.020 thing in the world. The second thing is that if you are persistently and continuously chronically
00:13:17.020 struggling to sleep, you don't have to because there are efficacious treatments, many of them
00:13:23.860 non-pharmacological, which is great, that can help course correct. In fact, even in older adults where
00:13:31.540 you think there is no hope at all for a solid night of sleep, those therapies, many of them seem to be
00:13:38.160 beneficial to restoring some degree of good sleep. So you don't have to suffer in the nighttime silence
00:13:44.820 that there is benefit there. I think that that's perhaps the best way to approach it with sensitivity,
00:13:51.960 compassion, understanding, but truthfulness to the science. You know, I wouldn't want to make people
00:13:59.240 nervous about, you know, eating so precisely that it doesn't change their blood sugar, set them on a path
00:14:06.900 towards, you know, pre-diabetes or type 2 diabetes. And where you become so obsessive and anxious that
00:14:14.520 food and the joy and pleasures of eating start to fail. I also don't want to do that with sleep,
00:14:21.700 but I equally don't want to tell you that it's fine just to eat a pint of ice cream every night and that
00:14:27.140 your blood sugar won't suffer. I'll tell you about that science too. Yeah, yeah. Well, so that's great
00:14:33.280 by way of introduction. And we will get into all of the aspects here, including all of the practical
00:14:39.200 recommendations you have for improving sleep and bypassing any perverse cul-de-sac of worry about
00:14:47.260 sleep that can get in the way of that project. So let's just begin. Let's jump into our first chapter
00:14:53.480 here on what sleep is, even before answering the question that is the title of your book about why we
00:15:01.500 sleep. What is sleep? From a functional perspective, I think the headline statement you could argue is
00:15:09.300 that sleep physiologically, at least, is perhaps the single most effective thing that we can do every
00:15:14.500 day to reset the health of our brain and our body. And that's not to dismiss food or nutrition or
00:15:22.280 exercise. But if you were to take you, Sam Harris, and I were to deprive you of food for 24 hours,
00:15:30.300 deprive you of water for 24 hours, deprive you of physical activity for 24 hours, or deprive you of
00:15:35.960 sleep for 24 hours, and I were to look across your brain and your body and see which one demonstrates
00:15:41.700 the more demonstrable impairment, by a very large margin, it's sleep. But I don't want to sort of do
00:15:48.520 that Coke, Pepsi, Dr. Pepper, I'm still missing one, I can't think of it, challenge. So you could ask
00:15:55.620 from a functional perspective what sleep is. You can also ask what is sleep as a process that unfolds
00:16:03.340 across the night in terms of its architecture. And then you can also ask and debate what is sleep as a
00:16:10.520 conscious state versus a non-conscious state. And so I'm happy to maybe speak about how sleep
00:16:18.120 unfolds, since that may be the logical entry point, or just go straight into how we can noodle and
00:16:24.420 wrestle with the idea of it being a conscious versus non-conscious state, which can get us into
00:16:28.420 tautological waters. But you tell me which of those two perhaps would be best to start with or fruitful
00:16:34.120 for you. Yeah, well, the question of whether it's conscious is, and I know I've spoken about this
00:16:40.940 elsewhere, is very difficult to resolve just because it's difficult to discriminate an interruption
00:16:47.840 in consciousness from a mere failure of memory. So for instance, dreams are routinely conscious,
00:16:55.020 but it's also possible to have dreams and not recall them at all. And then one could wonder
00:17:00.800 whether those dreams, you know, whether those stages of REM sleep were actually associated with
00:17:06.020 conscious dreaming. And one could wonder that the state of deep sleep is also a state of conscious
00:17:13.540 enjoyment of something quite formless and profound, but there's just no memory of it. And so we read it
00:17:22.800 as just a loss of experience for that period. And so I don't know how we would, I mean, I'm happy to hear
00:17:27.640 anything you think on that topic, but I'm unaware of anything that would resolve that for us.
00:17:33.320 I think it's a very elegant point, which is we rely for that question in part subjectively from the
00:17:42.460 sleeper themselves, a report of whether or not they were experiencing anything going through their mind
00:17:48.720 just before we woke them up and said, you know, what were you having as an experience? And that suffers from
00:17:56.180 the failures of memory, which we know happen. Just because you don't remember your dreams doesn't
00:18:02.380 mean that you weren't dreaming. I think one way that you can get closer, but we will still fail
00:18:07.700 is to split that question apart on the basis of perception, which is to say, depending on your,
00:18:19.200 I mean, behaviorally, the way that we define sleep in other species where we can't, for example,
00:18:23.660 stick electrodes on them is as a condition in which the organism stops responding to the outside world,
00:18:30.820 which is about perception. Does this mean that we are not conscious during sleep because we typically
00:18:36.240 stop responding to the outside world in all stages of sleep? And that depends on your definition of
00:18:42.200 consciousness, but we stop interacting with, and for the most part, perceiving the outside world,
00:18:49.720 which some would argue is a loss of consciousness, or at least a shift towards non-consciousness.
00:18:56.480 But I'd counter argue that we don't entirely stop perceiving the outside world. So for example,
00:19:03.420 I can have electrodes on your head and I can play sounds while you're asleep that don't wake you up.
00:19:09.240 And I can still see that the brain at some level is processing those sounds in a way that is not
00:19:15.360 dissimilar to the way it does when we're awake, consciously perceiving those sounds. We can do
00:19:21.120 fMRI studies and we can play those sounds as you're sleeping in the MRI scanner. It's hard to believe
00:19:27.180 that people can, but they do sleep in the scanner. And you can see that there are different ways of
00:19:32.480 perception. There's a great study that looked at new mothers. And what they found was that when they
00:19:38.300 played the cry of that infant versus another sound, even though they remained asleep, it was a very
00:19:45.680 different network, a salience network activated in response to the child of that mother versus
00:19:52.560 another sound of equal volume, et cetera. So there's definitely some degree of processing and
00:19:57.700 discriminatory processing, but I still don't think it's the same non-conscious state as anesthesia,
00:20:06.740 meaning that there is still some degree of perception of the outside world during deep
00:20:12.440 sleep. In other words, what we call extraception, the ability to focus or sense the outside world.
00:20:18.040 Well, there's got to be just based on the fact that you can wake somebody up from deep sleep,
00:20:23.840 you know, so that that's got to get in somehow.
00:20:25.900 That's exactly it. Yeah. Yeah. I think you, you exactly predicted where that conversation was going,
00:20:31.120 which is that no matter what stage you're in, sleep at least is a condition in which it is
00:20:36.700 environmentally reversible. For example, if a sound is loud enough or if someone were to pinch your
00:20:42.220 skin hard enough, which would be a desperately cruel thing, wouldn't it, to do when someone's
00:20:48.120 asleep, you would wake up from sleep, which is to say that in sleep, we are unresponsive,
00:20:53.780 but that state of unresponsivity is reversible. Now that's not true of anesthesia or death,
00:21:00.440 for as best we can tell. So I think it's very hard to argue then that we don't have a very
00:21:07.980 substantive yet qualitatively different form of consciousness when we dream, especially during
00:21:12.300 when we go into REM sleep dreaming. So I think we can get a little bit closer to a dissection of
00:21:17.940 what do we think of as the state of conscious processing during sleep. But I still feel as though
00:21:25.700 I don't see data that can really solidly give us one argument in either favor, conscious,
00:21:33.360 non-conscious state. Yeah. I would just add here that conversely, there are states of meditation or
00:21:40.820 drug intoxication where someone is also totally unresponsive to the outside world, but all too
00:21:48.580 conscious of something, right? I mean, in terms of their subjective report, once they come back from
00:21:54.560 those experiences. So there's kind of a double dissociation here. So I think responsiveness to
00:22:00.500 stimuli isn't the cut we need. We need, we obviously need the neural correlate of consciousness where we
00:22:07.340 can just scan your brain and say, you know, by some methodology and say, okay, this is the footprint
00:22:12.720 of consciousness in the human brain. And it winks out in this condition, let's say general anesthesia,
00:22:19.780 and it's attenuated to this degree in this stage of sleep. But unfortunately, we don't have that yet.
00:22:26.460 And I think there are conceptual and operational limits to our getting it. Again, the role of self-report
00:22:34.800 is always potentially confounding and seditious here because you can just, you know, we just need a
00:22:42.360 sufficient cohort of people who are reporting things that occurred in the chapter that we're deeming
00:22:50.220 to be unconscious. And, you know, either we're going to think they're delusional or they're lying or
00:22:57.320 they're in some other way wrong, or we're going to, or that's going to erode our confidence that
00:23:03.440 really the lights are out during that epoch.
00:23:06.180 I think self-report speaking about fickle mistresses is so prone to all of those errors.
00:23:13.120 Okay. So with that caveat in mind, let's launch into, it would be good to just give us the structure
00:23:19.580 of sleep here in human beings. You can say anything else you want about other animals, but what is sleep
00:23:27.200 for people?
00:23:29.040 Sleep, at least in human beings and in fact, in all mammalian species,
00:23:35.140 as long as they are land dwelling, there's a caveat there too, is broadly separated into two
00:23:40.620 main types. On the one hand, we have non-rapid eye movement sleep or non-REM sleep for short.
00:23:47.480 And on the other hand, we have rapid eye movement sleep or REM sleep. I often want to make people
00:23:54.400 clear on the fact that that's named not after the popular 1990s Michael Stipe pop band, but because
00:24:01.000 of these bizarre horizontal shuttling movements that occurred during this stage of sleep, that's where
00:24:07.280 it gets its definitional name from. And coming back to non-REM sleep, which I always feel sorry for,
00:24:14.940 by the way, isn't it sad to be defined by something that you're not? You are not REM sleep.
00:24:23.400 Well, I guess in this case, you're deep and light.
00:24:26.740 That's correct. So non-REM sleep is then further subdivided into four separate stages, increasing in
00:24:35.700 their depth of sleep. So stages one and two are what we would consider, or your sleep tracker will
00:24:40.660 probably try to tell you, are the light stages of non-REM sleep. Whereas stages three and four,
00:24:46.600 that's the really deep non-REM sleep. And REM sleep then is the stage in which we principally dream,
00:24:56.660 depending on your definition. Dreaming isn't exclusive to REM sleep, but for what most people
00:25:02.540 would say in the lay public, this is dreaming, what they're really referring to are the bizarre,
00:25:10.600 narrative, hallucinogenic, emotional, memory-laden experiences that come from this thing called REM
00:25:19.240 sleep. So those two types of sleep, non-REM and REM, will play out effectively in a battle for brain
00:25:27.820 domination throughout the night. And that cerebral war between non-REM and REM, in humans at least,
00:25:35.540 and it's different for different species, will last about 90 minutes. And that creates,
00:25:40.980 for the average adult, a prototypical 90-minute cycle where you go into non-REM sleep and then
00:25:46.760 you go into REM sleep. But what changes, however, is the ratio of non-REM to REM within those 90-minute
00:25:55.820 cycles as you move across the night. So in other words, in the first half of the night, the majority of
00:26:02.720 those 90-minute cycles are going to be comprised of lots of non-REM sleep, particularly deep non-REM
00:26:09.440 sleep. But as you push through to the second half of the night, that sort of seesaw balance shifts
00:26:16.220 over and those 90-minute cycles are comprised of much more rapid eye movement sleep and very little
00:26:23.320 deep sleep. And that has some consequences that we can also talk about. But I would probably mention
00:26:33.140 also every one of those stages of sleep, or almost all of those stages of sleep, we have now learned
00:26:40.460 are important. There is no one more important stage of sleep than the other. Now you can argue,
00:26:47.820 well, what are you talking about importance? Are you talking about mortality risk and death? And we can
00:26:51.820 use that as a filter to debate that as well. But overall, different stages of sleep provide
00:26:58.200 different functions for the brain and the body at different times of night. So we need all of those
00:27:04.640 stages. And is it true that we generally wake up, however briefly and indiscernible, after each of
00:27:13.640 these 90-minute phases? You get through your REM period and then there's a brief awakening?
00:27:18.800 That's absolutely, yeah. You definitely need to be a sleep researcher, take a sabbatical and...
00:27:25.460 Build me a time machine and I'll go back and have the conversation differently.
00:27:29.800 So we do know that usually at the end of every one of those 90-minute sleep cycles, at the end of
00:27:36.980 each of those REM phases, there is a brief termination of sleep where we wake up. And in part,
00:27:43.920 we think that that's perhaps because of the need to maneuver the body and change the body's
00:27:51.360 position. And so we have these brief awakenings. They're usually so brief that most of us don't
00:27:56.620 recall them. They're not imprinted in memory. But everyone will typically have a brief awakening and
00:28:02.760 then a movement episode after where they shift position.
00:28:06.060 Right. And we'll talk about sleep tracking and the tools that are available to do that
00:28:12.560 personally beyond going into a sleep lab and getting totally hooked up. But viewing these stages
00:28:20.980 in their totality, you've said that each is indispensable, but it does seem, at least in the
00:28:29.760 way one communicates the imperative to get all of these stages. Most of us are not deficient in
00:28:37.280 the stages of light sleep. And it's really the stages of REM and deep sleep that are marketed as
00:28:44.680 truly restorative, right? And those are the areas of real deficiency. I mean, so for instance,
00:28:50.560 if someone was sleeping six hours, but they got very long epochs of deep sleep and REM sleep,
00:29:02.140 would that strike you as a much healthier profile than someone sleeping six hours,
00:29:07.240 but it's mostly devoted to the stages one and two of light sleep?
00:29:12.180 Yes. I think that that's fair to say. We do need stage two as well. We've discovered that stage two
00:29:18.380 non-REM sleep is associated with certain forms of memory and memory processing. And there is a
00:29:25.400 particular electrical feature of stage two non-REM sleep, which continues on into deep non-REM sleep
00:29:32.520 stages three and four called sleep spindles, which are these beautiful little champagne cork
00:29:38.920 synchronous bursts of electrical activity that happen during stage two non-REM sleep. And then
00:29:45.220 stages three and four, they last for about a second and a second and a half. And they seem to be
00:29:50.880 critical for a number of different processes of both the brain and they seem to transact or be at
00:29:56.540 least associated with several benefits for the body. But overall, I would say that it's very difficult
00:30:02.920 to have a night where you're not transitioning because when you go down into deep non-REM sleep,
00:30:09.320 you have to progress through stage two. And when you're coming out of deep non-REM sleep,
00:30:15.220 you have to progress through stage two non-REM sleep again, the lighter form of non-REM sleep
00:30:20.200 before you get up into REM sleep. And so it would probably be rather difficult. You can manipulate
00:30:27.960 conditions in which this can happen, which I won't bore you with, but where you could have the scenario
00:30:33.940 that you described, but for the most part, you're still going to get that stage two non-REM sleep.
00:30:38.980 Yet what you said is correct.
00:30:41.720 Well, this is where I'll seed you with practical questions throughout. But the first that comes to
00:30:48.820 mind here is what are the implications of waking with an alarm clock versus waking with the change
00:30:58.520 in lighting conditions born of sunlight coming through the window? I guess there's the implication
00:31:04.440 of, uh, of using a sleep mask or a, uh, or blackout curtains where you're not getting those
00:31:11.160 environmental light cues. I can imagine, you know, if you're unlucky, your alarm clock rings
00:31:17.300 when you're in, in stage four sleep, say, and you're brought out of that in a less than ideal way.
00:31:24.020 What are, what, what are those, what are those effects and what do you actually recommend if
00:31:28.760 a person's schedule allows for it? What, what do you recommend as a, as a mode of, of waking up in
00:31:34.420 the morning? Unless you are waking up within the first couple of hours of sleep, it's unlikely that
00:31:42.620 your alarm would wake you up in the deep stages of non-REM sleep. That's not true. However, if you
00:31:48.360 take an afternoon nap and that nap lasts a little bit too long and by too long, what I mean is you're
00:31:55.440 going past that sort of 20 to 25 minutes and you're starting to go down into the deep sleep
00:31:59.940 and then your alarm wakes you up. Then you almost have this kind of sleep hangover for the next
00:32:08.880 hour or so. Those naps are terrible. Yeah. Like, you know, it's a change of time zone when you're,
00:32:14.400 when you have terrible jet lag and you decide, okay, there's no way I'm going to make it to
00:32:18.800 the evening. So I got to, I'm going to give myself an hour to sleep here. And that, that waking up
00:32:25.380 from that hour is just about the worst wake up one ever gets. It's pretty grim, isn't it? And it's
00:32:30.720 what we call sleep inertia where you get a state carryover where your brain never typically wakes
00:32:38.280 up from is jolted out of that deep sleep naturalistically from an evolutionary perspective
00:32:42.860 across millions of years. That's not been the case. And so we're not well prepared for
00:32:48.180 recovering from that assault. And therefore we suffer this terrible sleep inertia.
00:32:54.720 So it's not so likely to happen, but when it does happen, it's grim. It can also happen at night
00:33:01.240 when, for example, you get a phone call and all of a sudden it wakes you up at, you know,
00:33:07.220 2.30 or 1.30 in the morning. And once again, you're jolted out from that deep sleep. And yes,
00:33:14.020 you can answer the phone and you can be somewhat responsive, but it is just grim. You're in this
00:33:20.760 total treacle haze of cognitive dysfunction, and it's all you can do to allow words to tumble in
00:33:28.500 some meaningful way, one foot in front of the other out of your mouth.
00:33:31.420 So that is perhaps a less likely circumstance. What would I suggest? It's difficult because one
00:33:41.260 of the critical things that people need to do to get their sleep back on track is the simple act of
00:33:47.100 regularity, which is going to bed and waking up at the same time, no matter whether it's the weekday or
00:33:53.060 the weekend. And for that, we often require an alarm clock. And I also advocate for people not
00:34:02.820 just to have an alarm clock in the morning, but why don't we have a to-bed alarm as well as a to-wake
00:34:08.200 alarm? And it's one way to help keep us on schedule and track. I would say, however, that if you study
00:34:16.220 hunter-gatherer tribes whose way of life hasn't really changed for, you know, hundreds if not thousands of
00:34:21.840 years, they don't seem to wake up in an artificial manner. And if you ask them, you know, do you find
00:34:31.240 ways to force yourself to wake up? They find it a perplexing question. Why would you, why would you
00:34:38.460 terminate something that's not yet complete? It's a little bit like saying, why would you go out to
00:34:42.800 your favorite restaurant, order your favorite dish, have two bites of that dish, and then get up and walk
00:34:48.740 out? You would stay until you're full when you are complete with that meal. And why would we wake up
00:34:56.580 when we are not yet full of the sleep that we need? And Mother Nature will take care of that when it's
00:35:02.440 time to wake up and we've had the sleep that we need. We do. So one way some people will ask me,
00:35:08.320 how do I know if I'm getting enough sleep? It's not the ideal way, but one suggestion is to say,
00:35:14.580 if your alarm clock didn't go off in the morning, would you sleep past that alarm? And if the answer
00:35:18.840 is yes, then you're still carrying some degree of a sleep need, which means that by waking up
00:35:24.100 artificially, you're inducing a sleep debt as a consequence. What about the role of light cues in
00:35:30.600 bringing someone out of sleep? It's, we used to think that light perhaps was the trigger of,
00:35:38.280 or one of the facilitating functions for rising people out from sleep in the morning. And again,
00:35:47.760 by looking at those hunter-gatherer tribes, what we found is that that's not really the case. They
00:35:52.000 often typically will wake up a little bit before the dawn. What seems to be the trigger for the arrival
00:36:00.580 of wakefulness and the termination of sleep is more so temperature, both the internal temperature
00:36:06.420 and the ambient temperature rising, because often they will sleep with the environment,
00:36:13.220 with the ambient temperature, unlike many of us in modernity, where we have a controlled temperature.
00:36:21.060 So that's not to suggest that light can't be a facilitator to help you wake up in the morning.
00:36:27.120 And in fact, I will, I have one of these little smart lights next to my bedside,
00:36:32.480 and I program it to try and say, you know, two minutes before the time that you're supposed to
00:36:38.520 wake up, start to bring light into the room. I would say though, that I do have an alarm myself.
00:36:47.500 My alarm is, and we can get into sort of chronotypes and what your preference is, but
00:36:52.520 my alarm is set for around 7.04 in the morning or at 7.04 in the morning. Not because there's anything
00:36:59.260 special or unique. Please don't go rushing out and changing your wake-up time to that.
00:37:04.160 We're not going to have a chapter on numerology here and the significance of even numbers.
00:37:08.080 The only reason I do that is why not just be idiosyncratic? Why would you set it at,
00:37:13.440 you know, 7.05 or 7 or 7.10? Just why not 7.04? That tells you probably everything about me and
00:37:21.820 why I'm desperately unpopular. But so, but I usually wake up naturally, I would say about 80%
00:37:28.200 of the time I wake up naturally before my alarm clock. So I think there are, one of the worries
00:37:33.980 that people have when I tell them to do the experiment, if you have the luxury and the
00:37:37.500 schedule flexibility to do it, stop your alarm and just sleep in the way that you are, your body
00:37:45.660 wants to sleep. The greatest worry is that, my goodness, I normally wake up at 7 and I'll probably
00:37:52.100 wake up at 9 o'clock in the morning is the first concern. Now, that may be true to begin with for
00:37:58.200 the first few days because you're probably trying to sleep back a debt that you've amassed chronically
00:38:02.900 over weeks, if not months or years. And the second problem is that when people sleep long,
00:38:10.720 they wake up and once again, they have that strange sleep hangover effect where if they get
00:38:15.840 nine hours of sleep, they feel worse than when they get seven hours of sleep. That is typically
00:38:21.200 because you are in the phase of paying back the debt. And if you let that experiment play out for
00:38:27.380 another week, you wash away that sort of pressure to sleep. Now, we can speak about sleep debt and
00:38:33.320 whether you can ever truly pay back the bank or not. But that goes away with time. It's sort of like
00:38:40.380 detoxing from a drug. At first, it's brutal and you have all of these side effects and you have a
00:38:46.680 withdrawal syndrome. And in some ways, that's the withdrawal syndrome where you start sleeping longer.
00:38:51.880 That settles down. It's like a Richter shot and then it finds a sweet spot. And gradually,
00:38:57.320 you will actually acquiesce to your typical sleep need and your sleep profile. Most people don't have
00:39:02.080 the luxury to do that. So light can be helpful. Temperature is one. I also have one of those smart
00:39:08.500 home thermostats. And temperature is critical for sleep. We need to ironically warm up to cool down
00:39:19.220 to fall asleep. And then we need to stay cool to stay asleep. And finally, we need to warm up to wake
00:39:27.760 up. And so you can create a bespoke tailored temperature profile for your night of sleep that
00:39:35.800 can help to some degree. Now, of course, you're under the sheets and the ambient has some role to
00:39:41.640 play, but it's also altered by what's going on locally underneath the sheets too. So you can't
00:39:48.120 control it exquisitely. And that's where smart mattresses are coming in to try and take that out
00:39:52.460 the equation. So those are some of the ways that you can play around with sleep. I do like the idea
00:39:58.480 if you are particularly, if you are a night owl, and you struggle to wake up at the time that society
00:40:07.800 forces you to, which is not in synchrony with your morningness or eveningness preference,
00:40:14.320 you can use light in the morning. But then you can reverse that trick in the evening where you try to
00:40:20.960 ensconce yourself with as much dim light and darkness to help you try to get to bed a little bit
00:40:28.020 earlier. So it's not as though light should be dismissed and, you know, blocking devices,
00:40:34.120 blackout curtains, eye masks, earplugs, sound is another pollution that will disrupt your sleep.
00:40:40.640 I will typically use all of those. I have blackout curtains, I have an eye mask, and then I have
00:40:45.960 earplugs. I think I'm starting to sound like the Woody Allen neurotic of the sleep world, but that's
00:40:52.280 just me.
00:40:52.620 Yes. Well, all we need is one picture of this setup and to completely discredit you as a expert on sleep.
00:40:58.020 Oh, I've been so discredited by lots of different things, but that would, I think, seal the deal.
00:41:02.800 Okay, so let's transition to the question of why we sleep. I think there's probably no real boundary
00:41:10.760 between what sleep is and why we do it conceptually here, at least in places, because part of the story
00:41:18.360 here is the evolutionary question of just why sleep is a thing and how it came to be that animals like
00:41:27.440 ourselves dedicate so much of their lives to this state that seems fairly pointless and even dangerous.
00:41:35.600 I mean, this is the, you can imagine, in civilization, the danger is less salient, but just imagine
00:41:43.320 how precarious it would be to, you know, go out in the woods where there are bears and perhaps several
00:41:51.420 other species that could consider you a meal and to just take eight hours of darkness to be unconscious
00:42:01.120 for. I guess there's a potential evolutionary answer there in that the one thing you're not doing
00:42:07.780 when you're sleeping is stumbling around in the dark where you're not very good at seeing and
00:42:14.060 several other things can see you better than you can see them, but I'm not sure that's an adequate
00:42:18.900 rationale. So let's begin talking about the origins of sleep as we know them or can
00:42:28.860 hypothesize about them. What do you think about why sleep even exists?
00:42:35.780 So far in every species that we've studied to date, sleep or something that looks very much like
00:42:42.100 it seems to exist. And what that has suggested is that sleep evolved with life itself on this planet
00:42:51.060 and has fought its way through heroically every step along the evolutionary pathway.
00:42:56.980 Let's linger on that point. That's very interesting because you can imagine the adaptive benefits that
00:43:03.960 would generally accrue to any species that could just get over its need for sleep. I mean,
00:43:10.860 there would have been, you would think, a selective pressure in the direction of completely erasing
00:43:16.020 sleep. So it suggests that it's rather hard to do.
00:43:19.740 I think it's a beautiful way of thinking about it because from an evolutionary perspective,
00:43:25.280 just as you noted, it is the most idiotic of all things. Firstly, when you're asleep, you're not
00:43:30.540 eating, you're not foraging for food, you're not finding a mate, you're not reproducing, you're not
00:43:35.600 caring for your young. And worst of all, as you noted, you're vulnerable to predation. So on any one
00:43:41.140 of those grounds, but especially all of them as a collective, sleep should have been strongly
00:43:46.920 selected against during the course of evolution. And it's once been said that if sleep doesn't
00:43:53.840 serve an absolutely vital function, then it's the biggest mistake the evolutionary process ever made.
00:43:59.280 And what we've now since learned is that Mother Nature didn't make a spectacular blunder in creating
00:44:04.260 this thing called sleep. But even very old evolutionary, you know, species like earthworms,
00:44:11.520 for example, seem to have periods of, it's called lethargic, or essentially a sleep-like state.
00:44:18.940 You know, this takes sleep back millions of years. Even some bacteria that seem to live at least
00:44:25.220 several days, they will have an active phase and a passive phase, perhaps the precursor to sleep.
00:44:32.280 So you're right, you could well imagine why, if some species had understood a way to circumnavigate
00:44:41.260 its way around the essential need for sleep, it would have dominated for lots of different reasons,
00:44:46.880 at least within its species category. The fact that we haven't seen that yet argues that sleep must
00:44:53.600 be fundamental at the most basic of biological levels. And it's one of the reasons why when people
00:44:59.880 will say to me, well, look, can't you, you know, if you're a doctor training, I think we learned to
00:45:06.640 overcome our need for sleep. We learned to tolerate and deal with insufficient sleep, and you can do
00:45:13.220 that. If you could, trust me, I think Mother, you know, there's some degree of hubris there, which is
00:45:18.820 Mother Nature, if she could have even halved the amount of time that you are vulnerable to all of
00:45:25.860 those vicissitudes of sleep. She certainly would have. And the fact that it's been preserved tells
00:45:34.200 you that doesn't seem to be possible. And within the lifespan, we think that we can come along and
00:45:38.940 within a 10-year training of a career, we could overcome it. It's unlikely to be the case.
00:45:44.480 Actually, we might punctuate this part of the conversation with the cases of various people
00:45:51.460 who, at least by their own testimony, have gone a fair way toward overcoming their personal need
00:45:59.760 for sleep. I think it was Winston Churchill who, during the war years, was sleeping the last 10
00:46:05.960 minutes of every hour or something like that. I don't know if that's apocryphal, but what do we
00:46:11.340 know about anyone's successfully titrating their sleep down to something like a minimum? I'm sure
00:46:21.340 there are genotypes here that we may know something about where people just require less sleep than
00:46:26.360 is normal. But actually, I once had a doctor who claimed to sleep no more than three and a half hours
00:46:34.920 a night. And, you know, whether he was, again, this is before the age of sleep tracking, so he could
00:46:42.320 have been delusional. But what do we know about people who sleep much less than you would recommend?
00:46:50.220 Firstly, from an epidemiological or population-based perspective, which is simply associational,
00:46:55.240 using that sweet spot that we recommend, which is somewhere between seven to nine hours a night for
00:46:59.960 the average adult, once you start to get less than that, the shorter your sleep, the shorter your
00:47:05.380 life. That short sleep predicts or cause mortality. Are there people in history who have claimed to be
00:47:12.620 short sleepers? There are. And Churchill was one. Edison was another, although Edison was a habitual
00:47:19.200 napper during the day, and he used naps and sleep as a creative tool. Then you have Thatcher,
00:47:25.560 Margaret Thatcher, you have Ronald Reagan. Just named two people who ended their lives with
00:47:32.920 Alzheimer's. So that's not a great commercial for their strategies.
00:47:35.960 Exactly where I was going. Yeah. Yeah. You know, they seemed, if on the face of it,
00:47:41.700 to make it through until the, you know, 50s or even 60s. My goodness, there is evidential proof that you
00:47:49.040 can sleep what they claimed to be sleeping, which is four hours a night, and get away with it. And
00:47:54.860 ultimately, what we learned is that one way or another, sleep deficiency seems to get its hux into
00:48:02.940 you, that the elastic band of sleep deprivation can stretch only so far before it snaps. And
00:48:08.760 tragically, for both of those individuals, Thatcher and Reagan, they succumb to the disease of Alzheimer's.
00:48:17.080 And we now know that there are- I now realize we have several files open, but
00:48:20.300 each of these seems important. So on that point, how do we disentangle association and causation here?
00:48:29.380 Because couldn't it also be true that one of the early symptoms of Alzheimer's or, you know,
00:48:34.860 being at a special risk for it is to have one's apparent ability to sleep diminish over the course
00:48:41.880 of one's life, even maybe starting as early as one's 30s or 40s?
00:48:46.780 Yeah, so we can go, Alzheimer's disease is actually a great example. It's probably been,
00:48:52.000 I think, one of the most exciting areas of sleep research in terms of discoveries in the past 10
00:48:57.680 or even five years. We started with just those epidemiological associations, which are simply
00:49:03.260 that, the correlation, the not causation. And what that told us is that people who were
00:49:07.900 if you'd like to continue listening to this conversation, you'll need to subscribe at
00:49:16.020 samharris.org. Once you do, you'll get access to all full-length episodes of the Making Sense
00:49:20.680 podcast, along with other subscriber-only content, including bonus episodes and AMAs and the
00:49:26.960 conversations I've been having on the Waking Up app. The Making Sense podcast is ad-free and relies
00:49:32.260 entirely on listener support, and you can subscribe now at samharris.org.
00:49:37.900 Thank you.