#267 — The Kingdom of Sleep
Episode Stats
Words per Minute
156.33958
Summary
Matthew Walker is a professor of neuroscience and psychology at UC Berkeley and the Director of its Sleep and Neuroimaging Lab. He s also a former professor of psychiatry at Harvard University and has published over 100 scientific studies and has appeared on 60 Minutes, Nova, BBC News, and many other outlets. His first book, Why We Sleep, has been an international bestseller, and he also hosts his own podcast, The Matt Walker Podcast. In this episode, we discuss sleep and consciousness, the stages of sleep, the evolutionary origins of sleep and the generally doomed attempt to reduce one s need for sleep. We discuss sleep hygiene, caffeine and alcohol, sleep efficiency, bedtime restriction, napping, and finally, sleep tracking. And as we ll hear, each of us is associated with the company Aura, which makes a sleep tracking ring. And you ll hear about the connection between my own Aura Ring and Orthosomnia, which is a remarkable device I may have a love-hate relationship with. In any case, make of that what you will, and I hope you find this conversation useful as it runs nearly four hours. And we ll get into all of that in this episode of the Making Sense Podcast, hosted by Sam Harris. Thanks for joining me, it s a delight and a privilege to be speaking with you! Sam Harris - The Making Sense Podcatcher and in this podcast, by . by Matt Walker is a writer, researcher, and podcast host, and is a regular contributor to The New York Times, The New Yorker, The Huffington Post, and The Atlantic, and the New York Review, and so much more. Please consider becoming a supporter of what we re doing here. by becoming one of us, becoming a friend of us on the making sense podcast, by becoming a member of The Making sense Podcast on Insta- and we re making sense of it. , and we ll talk about it on social media, too, too of it, too , so you can help us make it so we do it on the Making sense of that, too we do that, and we get it like that, we can do it, we really do it all that we are making it so good, we like it, and that s not just that, so we know it, and they do it so much of it it s that, really are that
Transcript
00:00:00.000
Welcome to the Making Sense Podcast. This is Sam Harris. Just a note to say that if
00:00:12.180
you're hearing this, you are not currently on our subscriber feed and will only be hearing
00:00:16.320
the first part of this conversation. In order to access full episodes of the Making Sense
00:00:20.820
Podcast, you'll need to subscribe at samharris.org. There you'll find our private RSS feed to
00:00:26.420
add to your favorite podcatcher, along with other subscriber-only content. We don't run ads on the
00:00:31.580
podcast, and therefore it's made possible entirely through the support of our subscribers. So if you
00:00:36.360
enjoy what we're doing here, please consider becoming one. Today I'm speaking with Matthew
00:00:48.500
Walker. Matt is a professor of neuroscience and psychology at UC Berkeley and the director of
00:00:54.580
its Sleep and Neuroimaging Lab, and he's also a former professor of psychiatry at Harvard University.
00:01:00.680
He has published over 100 scientific studies and has appeared on 60 Minutes, Nova, BBC News,
00:01:08.080
and many other outlets. His first book, Why We Sleep, has been an international bestseller,
00:01:13.800
and he also hosts his own podcast, the Matt Walker Podcast. I've been wanting to speak to Matt for
00:01:20.980
quite some time because, as you'll hear, I've been increasingly worried about the quality of my own
00:01:25.520
sleep. I'm late to the party here, but now I'm convinced of the importance of sleeping well most
00:01:33.200
nights. And Matt and I get into all the details here about the nature and importance of sleep.
00:01:40.080
We discuss sleep and consciousness, the stages of sleep, sleep regularity, light and temperature,
00:01:47.820
the evolutionary origins of sleep, the generally doomed attempt to reduce one's need for sleep,
00:01:55.920
the connection between deficiencies in sleep and all-cause mortality, Alzheimer's disease, diabetes,
00:02:04.540
obesity, and heart disease, the role that sleep plays in learning and memory and mental health,
00:02:10.400
heart rate variability, REM sleep behavior disorder, and various parasomnias. We discuss lucid dreaming,
00:02:19.680
dreams as a kind of therapy, the connection between meditation and sleep, the various forms of insomnia,
00:02:27.580
and there are practical tips for what to do about them strewn throughout our conversation.
00:02:31.980
We discuss sleep hygiene, caffeine and alcohol, sleep efficiency, bedtime restriction,
00:02:40.920
napping, and finally sleep tracking. And as we'll hear on that final topic of sleep tracking,
00:02:48.700
Matt and I discover that each of us is associated with the company Aura that makes a sleep tracking ring.
00:02:54.920
I am a minor investor in the company, and Matt is its scientific advisor. Neither of us knew about
00:03:01.220
the connection before we started talking, and you'll hear I have a bit of a love-hate relationship
00:03:06.040
with my own Aura ring. It is a remarkable device, but I may have what Matt calls orthosomnia,
00:03:12.940
which is an overabundance of concern about my sleep data. In any case, make of that what you will,
00:03:19.160
and I hope you find this conversation useful as it runs nearly four hours. And now I bring you Matthew
00:03:26.340
Walker. I am here with Matthew Walker. Matt, thanks for joining me. It's a delight and a privilege to be
00:03:39.560
speaking with you, Sam. Thanks for having me. So you've written a book, Why We Sleep, that seems to
00:03:45.860
have gotten into the hands, if not the brains of more or less everyone. And now you have your own
00:03:53.040
podcast, the Matt Walker podcast. And you have been on many, many podcasts that I've noticed talking
00:04:01.040
about the science of sleep and seemingly almost single-handedly making people newly aware of the
00:04:13.060
importance of sleep in their lives, both from the side of, you know, physical health and mental health,
00:04:20.140
emotional regulation, really just across the board when you're talking about human well-being.
00:04:25.960
The difference between good and bad sleep seems paramount. And I must say, I have really neglected
00:04:33.460
sleep as a variable for most of my life. In fact, I think I was early in life toyed with the, you know,
00:04:43.140
fairly crazy ideal of limiting sleep so as to boost productivity. And we'll get into all of that. But
00:04:50.880
before we dive into the specific chapters of our conversation here, perhaps you can introduce
00:04:56.700
yourself, your background intellectually and academically, and just tell us how you came to
00:05:06.440
I wish I could take the compliment of bringing sleep back onto the public awareness map. I stand on the
00:05:13.580
shoulders of many of my colleagues and they are astronomically wonderful. So I try to do my part.
00:05:19.640
In terms of my background, I am a professor of neuroscience and psychology at the University
00:05:26.100
of California, Berkeley in America. And I've really tried to dedicate myself to understanding the
00:05:35.420
question of why we sleep for the past 20 years. I think like most people, I am an accidental sleep
00:05:42.800
researcher. I often think, you know, when kids are young and the teacher says, tell me what
00:05:49.620
you would like to be when you grow up. No one's shooting their hand up in the classroom and
00:05:54.320
saying, I desperately want to be a sleep researcher.
00:05:58.020
Yeah. And I can attest that when I started my neuroscience PhD, someone from a sleep lab,
00:06:04.620
I forget who, tried to recruit me to their lab. And I thought, why would I want to study sleep?
00:06:11.800
I had no interest at that point. And, you know, now I feel some chagrin over that dismissal because
00:06:19.940
it is increasingly fascinating. And as I said, consequential.
00:06:24.280
And in some ways, I, you know, I don't blame you. Maybe at the time, certainly even 20 years ago,
00:06:28.860
one could argue it's almost academic suicide to suggest that you want to become a sleep researcher
00:06:34.600
and not necessarily truthful, but some would argue that it was almost a charlatan science
00:06:41.480
to begin with. And of course it is. It's the most bizarre, strange, illogical, irrational,
00:06:48.620
from an evolutionary perspective, idiotic thing that an organism can do. And you're going to
00:06:55.120
leverage an entire academic career on that platform. Good luck and good night would be the,
00:07:00.720
I think, the tagline. But I was studying for my PhD, people with different forms of dementia.
00:07:09.280
And I was using brainwave patterns to try and differentially diagnose them very early on in
00:07:14.060
the course of dementia. And I was failing miserably, couldn't get any good results.
00:07:19.480
And one weekend, I had this little igloo of journals that I would retreat to, which tells you
00:07:25.000
everything about my social life. And I started to learn that some of those dementias
00:07:30.540
would eat away at sleep centers and other forms of the dementias would not, because there are many
00:07:35.820
different forms of dementia. So I realized I was measuring my patients at the wrong time,
00:07:39.960
which was when they were awake, and I should be measuring them when they were asleep.
00:07:44.100
I started doing that. I got some fantastic results. And at that point, I started to ask the question,
00:07:51.520
I wonder if these sleep disruptions and impairments are not a consequence of the dementia.
00:07:58.780
They're not a symptom of the dementia. Maybe they are a cause of the dementia. But I realized 20 years
00:08:05.320
ago, no one could answer a very fundamental question, which was, why do we sleep? And I think
00:08:10.860
the crass answer at that time was that we sleep to cure sleepiness, which is the fatuous equivalent of
00:08:18.660
saying, I eat to cure hunger. It tells you nothing about the unique benefits.
00:08:23.240
But then I started to explore this thing called sleep, and I fell absolutely in love with it. And to
00:08:33.220
this day, 20 years on, I still think it is the most beguiling thing in science. It is a love affair that's
00:08:39.840
not left me for all of those decades. And I remain an amorous partner to its wonderful gifts, both
00:08:48.520
nightly as a practice and also from an intellectual and academic and research perspective. Does that
00:08:56.000
Yeah, yeah. Well, if I can follow your romantic analogy here, sleep is a fairly coy mistress for
00:09:04.300
many of us. And this, you know, speaking personally, this has always been not even on the back burner for
00:09:13.780
me as a problem to solve in my life. I just, I've accustomed myself to sleeping badly and just accepting
00:09:23.000
on some level that I sleep badly. And so, encountering your work is fairly arresting to someone in my
00:09:31.440
condition, because the stakes, as we will elucidate here, are incredibly high, given the connection between
00:09:39.500
sleep and health. So, I wanted to, at the outset, address the component of worry here, worry about
00:09:48.720
sleep, because many people listening to us will also recognize in themselves that their sleep is
00:09:55.340
far from ideal. And to add a layer of worry to that is obviously counterproductive when the goal is to
00:10:05.560
make it easier to sleep soundly and on some better schedule in general. So, can you address this effect
00:10:14.420
that our conversation is likely to have, especially when we're talking about possible links between
00:10:20.900
poor sleep and dementia and, you know, all the rest? It's just, it's very easy to begin to treat
00:10:28.440
this as some kind of medical emergency in the offing. What do you have to say as a, by way of
00:10:37.660
In some ways, it's a rock and a hard place that I've found myself in. And this is something that
00:10:43.920
I've learned since publishing the book. And I think it's something that I've corrected in my
00:10:48.600
communication to the public. As I was writing the book at the time, at least within the public sphere,
00:10:54.300
as you mentioned, sleep was the neglected step system in the health conversation of today. And it
00:11:00.380
was that way. And I was so familiar, as all of my colleagues were, with the disease and the sickness
00:11:07.500
and the suffering that was happening because of this sleep deficiency that was so pernicious throughout
00:11:14.120
most first world nations, that I wanted to try to, no pun intended for either this podcast or the
00:11:24.060
topic, but sort of wake people up to the fact of the importance of sleep. And I think that in my
00:11:30.300
communications and maybe even in segments of the book, I was perhaps heavy handed. And I had neglected
00:11:38.580
to recognize the concern for the sleep anxious and those who are having sleep difficulty. And I've
00:11:46.560
since become so much more sensitive to that. And I can't deny the science. I can't not tell you
00:11:56.920
about the links between insufficient sleep and Alzheimer's disease, obesity, diabetes, cardiovascular
00:12:04.580
disease, depression, anxiety, even suicide, some forms of cancer. But I also don't want people to
00:12:12.520
become overly anxious. But how do you do that? How do you find that sweet spot? And so for me, it's
00:12:20.540
been a real lesson and a lesson also because I am no poster child for sleep. I have had my battles and
00:12:27.560
I did not mention them in the book. And I think I should have. I'm being personally open. I'm a very
00:12:33.200
private person. I've had at least three bouts of insomnia during my lifetime and they were vicious.
00:12:39.740
And just because you know a little about sleep doesn't mean as though you are immune to its
00:12:45.860
vagaries. It is a mistress that can be very fickle. So I think for this podcast, it's important to keep
00:12:54.620
in mind two things. First, everyone has a bad night of sleep. And if you're there at night struggling to fall
00:13:03.480
asleep, don't worry. Even with all of the facts and the science that we will discuss, it's not the worst
00:13:11.020
thing in the world. The second thing is that if you are persistently and continuously chronically
00:13:17.020
struggling to sleep, you don't have to because there are efficacious treatments, many of them
00:13:23.860
non-pharmacological, which is great, that can help course correct. In fact, even in older adults where
00:13:31.540
you think there is no hope at all for a solid night of sleep, those therapies, many of them seem to be
00:13:38.160
beneficial to restoring some degree of good sleep. So you don't have to suffer in the nighttime silence
00:13:44.820
that there is benefit there. I think that that's perhaps the best way to approach it with sensitivity,
00:13:51.960
compassion, understanding, but truthfulness to the science. You know, I wouldn't want to make people
00:13:59.240
nervous about, you know, eating so precisely that it doesn't change their blood sugar, set them on a path
00:14:06.900
towards, you know, pre-diabetes or type 2 diabetes. And where you become so obsessive and anxious that
00:14:14.520
food and the joy and pleasures of eating start to fail. I also don't want to do that with sleep,
00:14:21.700
but I equally don't want to tell you that it's fine just to eat a pint of ice cream every night and that
00:14:27.140
your blood sugar won't suffer. I'll tell you about that science too. Yeah, yeah. Well, so that's great
00:14:33.280
by way of introduction. And we will get into all of the aspects here, including all of the practical
00:14:39.200
recommendations you have for improving sleep and bypassing any perverse cul-de-sac of worry about
00:14:47.260
sleep that can get in the way of that project. So let's just begin. Let's jump into our first chapter
00:14:53.480
here on what sleep is, even before answering the question that is the title of your book about why we
00:15:01.500
sleep. What is sleep? From a functional perspective, I think the headline statement you could argue is
00:15:09.300
that sleep physiologically, at least, is perhaps the single most effective thing that we can do every
00:15:14.500
day to reset the health of our brain and our body. And that's not to dismiss food or nutrition or
00:15:22.280
exercise. But if you were to take you, Sam Harris, and I were to deprive you of food for 24 hours,
00:15:30.300
deprive you of water for 24 hours, deprive you of physical activity for 24 hours, or deprive you of
00:15:35.960
sleep for 24 hours, and I were to look across your brain and your body and see which one demonstrates
00:15:41.700
the more demonstrable impairment, by a very large margin, it's sleep. But I don't want to sort of do
00:15:48.520
that Coke, Pepsi, Dr. Pepper, I'm still missing one, I can't think of it, challenge. So you could ask
00:15:55.620
from a functional perspective what sleep is. You can also ask what is sleep as a process that unfolds
00:16:03.340
across the night in terms of its architecture. And then you can also ask and debate what is sleep as a
00:16:10.520
conscious state versus a non-conscious state. And so I'm happy to maybe speak about how sleep
00:16:18.120
unfolds, since that may be the logical entry point, or just go straight into how we can noodle and
00:16:24.420
wrestle with the idea of it being a conscious versus non-conscious state, which can get us into
00:16:28.420
tautological waters. But you tell me which of those two perhaps would be best to start with or fruitful
00:16:34.120
for you. Yeah, well, the question of whether it's conscious is, and I know I've spoken about this
00:16:40.940
elsewhere, is very difficult to resolve just because it's difficult to discriminate an interruption
00:16:47.840
in consciousness from a mere failure of memory. So for instance, dreams are routinely conscious,
00:16:55.020
but it's also possible to have dreams and not recall them at all. And then one could wonder
00:17:00.800
whether those dreams, you know, whether those stages of REM sleep were actually associated with
00:17:06.020
conscious dreaming. And one could wonder that the state of deep sleep is also a state of conscious
00:17:13.540
enjoyment of something quite formless and profound, but there's just no memory of it. And so we read it
00:17:22.800
as just a loss of experience for that period. And so I don't know how we would, I mean, I'm happy to hear
00:17:27.640
anything you think on that topic, but I'm unaware of anything that would resolve that for us.
00:17:33.320
I think it's a very elegant point, which is we rely for that question in part subjectively from the
00:17:42.460
sleeper themselves, a report of whether or not they were experiencing anything going through their mind
00:17:48.720
just before we woke them up and said, you know, what were you having as an experience? And that suffers from
00:17:56.180
the failures of memory, which we know happen. Just because you don't remember your dreams doesn't
00:18:02.380
mean that you weren't dreaming. I think one way that you can get closer, but we will still fail
00:18:07.700
is to split that question apart on the basis of perception, which is to say, depending on your,
00:18:19.200
I mean, behaviorally, the way that we define sleep in other species where we can't, for example,
00:18:23.660
stick electrodes on them is as a condition in which the organism stops responding to the outside world,
00:18:30.820
which is about perception. Does this mean that we are not conscious during sleep because we typically
00:18:36.240
stop responding to the outside world in all stages of sleep? And that depends on your definition of
00:18:42.200
consciousness, but we stop interacting with, and for the most part, perceiving the outside world,
00:18:49.720
which some would argue is a loss of consciousness, or at least a shift towards non-consciousness.
00:18:56.480
But I'd counter argue that we don't entirely stop perceiving the outside world. So for example,
00:19:03.420
I can have electrodes on your head and I can play sounds while you're asleep that don't wake you up.
00:19:09.240
And I can still see that the brain at some level is processing those sounds in a way that is not
00:19:15.360
dissimilar to the way it does when we're awake, consciously perceiving those sounds. We can do
00:19:21.120
fMRI studies and we can play those sounds as you're sleeping in the MRI scanner. It's hard to believe
00:19:27.180
that people can, but they do sleep in the scanner. And you can see that there are different ways of
00:19:32.480
perception. There's a great study that looked at new mothers. And what they found was that when they
00:19:38.300
played the cry of that infant versus another sound, even though they remained asleep, it was a very
00:19:45.680
different network, a salience network activated in response to the child of that mother versus
00:19:52.560
another sound of equal volume, et cetera. So there's definitely some degree of processing and
00:19:57.700
discriminatory processing, but I still don't think it's the same non-conscious state as anesthesia,
00:20:06.740
meaning that there is still some degree of perception of the outside world during deep
00:20:12.440
sleep. In other words, what we call extraception, the ability to focus or sense the outside world.
00:20:18.040
Well, there's got to be just based on the fact that you can wake somebody up from deep sleep,
00:20:23.840
you know, so that that's got to get in somehow.
00:20:25.900
That's exactly it. Yeah. Yeah. I think you, you exactly predicted where that conversation was going,
00:20:31.120
which is that no matter what stage you're in, sleep at least is a condition in which it is
00:20:36.700
environmentally reversible. For example, if a sound is loud enough or if someone were to pinch your
00:20:42.220
skin hard enough, which would be a desperately cruel thing, wouldn't it, to do when someone's
00:20:48.120
asleep, you would wake up from sleep, which is to say that in sleep, we are unresponsive,
00:20:53.780
but that state of unresponsivity is reversible. Now that's not true of anesthesia or death,
00:21:00.440
for as best we can tell. So I think it's very hard to argue then that we don't have a very
00:21:07.980
substantive yet qualitatively different form of consciousness when we dream, especially during
00:21:12.300
when we go into REM sleep dreaming. So I think we can get a little bit closer to a dissection of
00:21:17.940
what do we think of as the state of conscious processing during sleep. But I still feel as though
00:21:25.700
I don't see data that can really solidly give us one argument in either favor, conscious,
00:21:33.360
non-conscious state. Yeah. I would just add here that conversely, there are states of meditation or
00:21:40.820
drug intoxication where someone is also totally unresponsive to the outside world, but all too
00:21:48.580
conscious of something, right? I mean, in terms of their subjective report, once they come back from
00:21:54.560
those experiences. So there's kind of a double dissociation here. So I think responsiveness to
00:22:00.500
stimuli isn't the cut we need. We need, we obviously need the neural correlate of consciousness where we
00:22:07.340
can just scan your brain and say, you know, by some methodology and say, okay, this is the footprint
00:22:12.720
of consciousness in the human brain. And it winks out in this condition, let's say general anesthesia,
00:22:19.780
and it's attenuated to this degree in this stage of sleep. But unfortunately, we don't have that yet.
00:22:26.460
And I think there are conceptual and operational limits to our getting it. Again, the role of self-report
00:22:34.800
is always potentially confounding and seditious here because you can just, you know, we just need a
00:22:42.360
sufficient cohort of people who are reporting things that occurred in the chapter that we're deeming
00:22:50.220
to be unconscious. And, you know, either we're going to think they're delusional or they're lying or
00:22:57.320
they're in some other way wrong, or we're going to, or that's going to erode our confidence that
00:23:06.180
I think self-report speaking about fickle mistresses is so prone to all of those errors.
00:23:13.120
Okay. So with that caveat in mind, let's launch into, it would be good to just give us the structure
00:23:19.580
of sleep here in human beings. You can say anything else you want about other animals, but what is sleep
00:23:29.040
Sleep, at least in human beings and in fact, in all mammalian species,
00:23:35.140
as long as they are land dwelling, there's a caveat there too, is broadly separated into two
00:23:40.620
main types. On the one hand, we have non-rapid eye movement sleep or non-REM sleep for short.
00:23:47.480
And on the other hand, we have rapid eye movement sleep or REM sleep. I often want to make people
00:23:54.400
clear on the fact that that's named not after the popular 1990s Michael Stipe pop band, but because
00:24:01.000
of these bizarre horizontal shuttling movements that occurred during this stage of sleep, that's where
00:24:07.280
it gets its definitional name from. And coming back to non-REM sleep, which I always feel sorry for,
00:24:14.940
by the way, isn't it sad to be defined by something that you're not? You are not REM sleep.
00:24:23.400
Well, I guess in this case, you're deep and light.
00:24:26.740
That's correct. So non-REM sleep is then further subdivided into four separate stages, increasing in
00:24:35.700
their depth of sleep. So stages one and two are what we would consider, or your sleep tracker will
00:24:40.660
probably try to tell you, are the light stages of non-REM sleep. Whereas stages three and four,
00:24:46.600
that's the really deep non-REM sleep. And REM sleep then is the stage in which we principally dream,
00:24:56.660
depending on your definition. Dreaming isn't exclusive to REM sleep, but for what most people
00:25:02.540
would say in the lay public, this is dreaming, what they're really referring to are the bizarre,
00:25:10.600
narrative, hallucinogenic, emotional, memory-laden experiences that come from this thing called REM
00:25:19.240
sleep. So those two types of sleep, non-REM and REM, will play out effectively in a battle for brain
00:25:27.820
domination throughout the night. And that cerebral war between non-REM and REM, in humans at least,
00:25:35.540
and it's different for different species, will last about 90 minutes. And that creates,
00:25:40.980
for the average adult, a prototypical 90-minute cycle where you go into non-REM sleep and then
00:25:46.760
you go into REM sleep. But what changes, however, is the ratio of non-REM to REM within those 90-minute
00:25:55.820
cycles as you move across the night. So in other words, in the first half of the night, the majority of
00:26:02.720
those 90-minute cycles are going to be comprised of lots of non-REM sleep, particularly deep non-REM
00:26:09.440
sleep. But as you push through to the second half of the night, that sort of seesaw balance shifts
00:26:16.220
over and those 90-minute cycles are comprised of much more rapid eye movement sleep and very little
00:26:23.320
deep sleep. And that has some consequences that we can also talk about. But I would probably mention
00:26:33.140
also every one of those stages of sleep, or almost all of those stages of sleep, we have now learned
00:26:40.460
are important. There is no one more important stage of sleep than the other. Now you can argue,
00:26:47.820
well, what are you talking about importance? Are you talking about mortality risk and death? And we can
00:26:51.820
use that as a filter to debate that as well. But overall, different stages of sleep provide
00:26:58.200
different functions for the brain and the body at different times of night. So we need all of those
00:27:04.640
stages. And is it true that we generally wake up, however briefly and indiscernible, after each of
00:27:13.640
these 90-minute phases? You get through your REM period and then there's a brief awakening?
00:27:18.800
That's absolutely, yeah. You definitely need to be a sleep researcher, take a sabbatical and...
00:27:25.460
Build me a time machine and I'll go back and have the conversation differently.
00:27:29.800
So we do know that usually at the end of every one of those 90-minute sleep cycles, at the end of
00:27:36.980
each of those REM phases, there is a brief termination of sleep where we wake up. And in part,
00:27:43.920
we think that that's perhaps because of the need to maneuver the body and change the body's
00:27:51.360
position. And so we have these brief awakenings. They're usually so brief that most of us don't
00:27:56.620
recall them. They're not imprinted in memory. But everyone will typically have a brief awakening and
00:28:02.760
then a movement episode after where they shift position.
00:28:06.060
Right. And we'll talk about sleep tracking and the tools that are available to do that
00:28:12.560
personally beyond going into a sleep lab and getting totally hooked up. But viewing these stages
00:28:20.980
in their totality, you've said that each is indispensable, but it does seem, at least in the
00:28:29.760
way one communicates the imperative to get all of these stages. Most of us are not deficient in
00:28:37.280
the stages of light sleep. And it's really the stages of REM and deep sleep that are marketed as
00:28:44.680
truly restorative, right? And those are the areas of real deficiency. I mean, so for instance,
00:28:50.560
if someone was sleeping six hours, but they got very long epochs of deep sleep and REM sleep,
00:29:02.140
would that strike you as a much healthier profile than someone sleeping six hours,
00:29:07.240
but it's mostly devoted to the stages one and two of light sleep?
00:29:12.180
Yes. I think that that's fair to say. We do need stage two as well. We've discovered that stage two
00:29:18.380
non-REM sleep is associated with certain forms of memory and memory processing. And there is a
00:29:25.400
particular electrical feature of stage two non-REM sleep, which continues on into deep non-REM sleep
00:29:32.520
stages three and four called sleep spindles, which are these beautiful little champagne cork
00:29:38.920
synchronous bursts of electrical activity that happen during stage two non-REM sleep. And then
00:29:45.220
stages three and four, they last for about a second and a second and a half. And they seem to be
00:29:50.880
critical for a number of different processes of both the brain and they seem to transact or be at
00:29:56.540
least associated with several benefits for the body. But overall, I would say that it's very difficult
00:30:02.920
to have a night where you're not transitioning because when you go down into deep non-REM sleep,
00:30:09.320
you have to progress through stage two. And when you're coming out of deep non-REM sleep,
00:30:15.220
you have to progress through stage two non-REM sleep again, the lighter form of non-REM sleep
00:30:20.200
before you get up into REM sleep. And so it would probably be rather difficult. You can manipulate
00:30:27.960
conditions in which this can happen, which I won't bore you with, but where you could have the scenario
00:30:33.940
that you described, but for the most part, you're still going to get that stage two non-REM sleep.
00:30:41.720
Well, this is where I'll seed you with practical questions throughout. But the first that comes to
00:30:48.820
mind here is what are the implications of waking with an alarm clock versus waking with the change
00:30:58.520
in lighting conditions born of sunlight coming through the window? I guess there's the implication
00:31:04.440
of, uh, of using a sleep mask or a, uh, or blackout curtains where you're not getting those
00:31:11.160
environmental light cues. I can imagine, you know, if you're unlucky, your alarm clock rings
00:31:17.300
when you're in, in stage four sleep, say, and you're brought out of that in a less than ideal way.
00:31:24.020
What are, what, what are those, what are those effects and what do you actually recommend if
00:31:28.760
a person's schedule allows for it? What, what do you recommend as a, as a mode of, of waking up in
00:31:34.420
the morning? Unless you are waking up within the first couple of hours of sleep, it's unlikely that
00:31:42.620
your alarm would wake you up in the deep stages of non-REM sleep. That's not true. However, if you
00:31:48.360
take an afternoon nap and that nap lasts a little bit too long and by too long, what I mean is you're
00:31:55.440
going past that sort of 20 to 25 minutes and you're starting to go down into the deep sleep
00:31:59.940
and then your alarm wakes you up. Then you almost have this kind of sleep hangover for the next
00:32:08.880
hour or so. Those naps are terrible. Yeah. Like, you know, it's a change of time zone when you're,
00:32:14.400
when you have terrible jet lag and you decide, okay, there's no way I'm going to make it to
00:32:18.800
the evening. So I got to, I'm going to give myself an hour to sleep here. And that, that waking up
00:32:25.380
from that hour is just about the worst wake up one ever gets. It's pretty grim, isn't it? And it's
00:32:30.720
what we call sleep inertia where you get a state carryover where your brain never typically wakes
00:32:38.280
up from is jolted out of that deep sleep naturalistically from an evolutionary perspective
00:32:42.860
across millions of years. That's not been the case. And so we're not well prepared for
00:32:48.180
recovering from that assault. And therefore we suffer this terrible sleep inertia.
00:32:54.720
So it's not so likely to happen, but when it does happen, it's grim. It can also happen at night
00:33:01.240
when, for example, you get a phone call and all of a sudden it wakes you up at, you know,
00:33:07.220
2.30 or 1.30 in the morning. And once again, you're jolted out from that deep sleep. And yes,
00:33:14.020
you can answer the phone and you can be somewhat responsive, but it is just grim. You're in this
00:33:20.760
total treacle haze of cognitive dysfunction, and it's all you can do to allow words to tumble in
00:33:28.500
some meaningful way, one foot in front of the other out of your mouth.
00:33:31.420
So that is perhaps a less likely circumstance. What would I suggest? It's difficult because one
00:33:41.260
of the critical things that people need to do to get their sleep back on track is the simple act of
00:33:47.100
regularity, which is going to bed and waking up at the same time, no matter whether it's the weekday or
00:33:53.060
the weekend. And for that, we often require an alarm clock. And I also advocate for people not
00:34:02.820
just to have an alarm clock in the morning, but why don't we have a to-bed alarm as well as a to-wake
00:34:08.200
alarm? And it's one way to help keep us on schedule and track. I would say, however, that if you study
00:34:16.220
hunter-gatherer tribes whose way of life hasn't really changed for, you know, hundreds if not thousands of
00:34:21.840
years, they don't seem to wake up in an artificial manner. And if you ask them, you know, do you find
00:34:31.240
ways to force yourself to wake up? They find it a perplexing question. Why would you, why would you
00:34:38.460
terminate something that's not yet complete? It's a little bit like saying, why would you go out to
00:34:42.800
your favorite restaurant, order your favorite dish, have two bites of that dish, and then get up and walk
00:34:48.740
out? You would stay until you're full when you are complete with that meal. And why would we wake up
00:34:56.580
when we are not yet full of the sleep that we need? And Mother Nature will take care of that when it's
00:35:02.440
time to wake up and we've had the sleep that we need. We do. So one way some people will ask me,
00:35:08.320
how do I know if I'm getting enough sleep? It's not the ideal way, but one suggestion is to say,
00:35:14.580
if your alarm clock didn't go off in the morning, would you sleep past that alarm? And if the answer
00:35:18.840
is yes, then you're still carrying some degree of a sleep need, which means that by waking up
00:35:24.100
artificially, you're inducing a sleep debt as a consequence. What about the role of light cues in
00:35:30.600
bringing someone out of sleep? It's, we used to think that light perhaps was the trigger of,
00:35:38.280
or one of the facilitating functions for rising people out from sleep in the morning. And again,
00:35:47.760
by looking at those hunter-gatherer tribes, what we found is that that's not really the case. They
00:35:52.000
often typically will wake up a little bit before the dawn. What seems to be the trigger for the arrival
00:36:00.580
of wakefulness and the termination of sleep is more so temperature, both the internal temperature
00:36:06.420
and the ambient temperature rising, because often they will sleep with the environment,
00:36:13.220
with the ambient temperature, unlike many of us in modernity, where we have a controlled temperature.
00:36:21.060
So that's not to suggest that light can't be a facilitator to help you wake up in the morning.
00:36:27.120
And in fact, I will, I have one of these little smart lights next to my bedside,
00:36:32.480
and I program it to try and say, you know, two minutes before the time that you're supposed to
00:36:38.520
wake up, start to bring light into the room. I would say though, that I do have an alarm myself.
00:36:47.500
My alarm is, and we can get into sort of chronotypes and what your preference is, but
00:36:52.520
my alarm is set for around 7.04 in the morning or at 7.04 in the morning. Not because there's anything
00:36:59.260
special or unique. Please don't go rushing out and changing your wake-up time to that.
00:37:04.160
We're not going to have a chapter on numerology here and the significance of even numbers.
00:37:08.080
The only reason I do that is why not just be idiosyncratic? Why would you set it at,
00:37:13.440
you know, 7.05 or 7 or 7.10? Just why not 7.04? That tells you probably everything about me and
00:37:21.820
why I'm desperately unpopular. But so, but I usually wake up naturally, I would say about 80%
00:37:28.200
of the time I wake up naturally before my alarm clock. So I think there are, one of the worries
00:37:33.980
that people have when I tell them to do the experiment, if you have the luxury and the
00:37:37.500
schedule flexibility to do it, stop your alarm and just sleep in the way that you are, your body
00:37:45.660
wants to sleep. The greatest worry is that, my goodness, I normally wake up at 7 and I'll probably
00:37:52.100
wake up at 9 o'clock in the morning is the first concern. Now, that may be true to begin with for
00:37:58.200
the first few days because you're probably trying to sleep back a debt that you've amassed chronically
00:38:02.900
over weeks, if not months or years. And the second problem is that when people sleep long,
00:38:10.720
they wake up and once again, they have that strange sleep hangover effect where if they get
00:38:15.840
nine hours of sleep, they feel worse than when they get seven hours of sleep. That is typically
00:38:21.200
because you are in the phase of paying back the debt. And if you let that experiment play out for
00:38:27.380
another week, you wash away that sort of pressure to sleep. Now, we can speak about sleep debt and
00:38:33.320
whether you can ever truly pay back the bank or not. But that goes away with time. It's sort of like
00:38:40.380
detoxing from a drug. At first, it's brutal and you have all of these side effects and you have a
00:38:46.680
withdrawal syndrome. And in some ways, that's the withdrawal syndrome where you start sleeping longer.
00:38:51.880
That settles down. It's like a Richter shot and then it finds a sweet spot. And gradually,
00:38:57.320
you will actually acquiesce to your typical sleep need and your sleep profile. Most people don't have
00:39:02.080
the luxury to do that. So light can be helpful. Temperature is one. I also have one of those smart
00:39:08.500
home thermostats. And temperature is critical for sleep. We need to ironically warm up to cool down
00:39:19.220
to fall asleep. And then we need to stay cool to stay asleep. And finally, we need to warm up to wake
00:39:27.760
up. And so you can create a bespoke tailored temperature profile for your night of sleep that
00:39:35.800
can help to some degree. Now, of course, you're under the sheets and the ambient has some role to
00:39:41.640
play, but it's also altered by what's going on locally underneath the sheets too. So you can't
00:39:48.120
control it exquisitely. And that's where smart mattresses are coming in to try and take that out
00:39:52.460
the equation. So those are some of the ways that you can play around with sleep. I do like the idea
00:39:58.480
if you are particularly, if you are a night owl, and you struggle to wake up at the time that society
00:40:07.800
forces you to, which is not in synchrony with your morningness or eveningness preference,
00:40:14.320
you can use light in the morning. But then you can reverse that trick in the evening where you try to
00:40:20.960
ensconce yourself with as much dim light and darkness to help you try to get to bed a little bit
00:40:28.020
earlier. So it's not as though light should be dismissed and, you know, blocking devices,
00:40:34.120
blackout curtains, eye masks, earplugs, sound is another pollution that will disrupt your sleep.
00:40:40.640
I will typically use all of those. I have blackout curtains, I have an eye mask, and then I have
00:40:45.960
earplugs. I think I'm starting to sound like the Woody Allen neurotic of the sleep world, but that's
00:40:52.620
Yes. Well, all we need is one picture of this setup and to completely discredit you as a expert on sleep.
00:40:58.020
Oh, I've been so discredited by lots of different things, but that would, I think, seal the deal.
00:41:02.800
Okay, so let's transition to the question of why we sleep. I think there's probably no real boundary
00:41:10.760
between what sleep is and why we do it conceptually here, at least in places, because part of the story
00:41:18.360
here is the evolutionary question of just why sleep is a thing and how it came to be that animals like
00:41:27.440
ourselves dedicate so much of their lives to this state that seems fairly pointless and even dangerous.
00:41:35.600
I mean, this is the, you can imagine, in civilization, the danger is less salient, but just imagine
00:41:43.320
how precarious it would be to, you know, go out in the woods where there are bears and perhaps several
00:41:51.420
other species that could consider you a meal and to just take eight hours of darkness to be unconscious
00:42:01.120
for. I guess there's a potential evolutionary answer there in that the one thing you're not doing
00:42:07.780
when you're sleeping is stumbling around in the dark where you're not very good at seeing and
00:42:14.060
several other things can see you better than you can see them, but I'm not sure that's an adequate
00:42:18.900
rationale. So let's begin talking about the origins of sleep as we know them or can
00:42:28.860
hypothesize about them. What do you think about why sleep even exists?
00:42:35.780
So far in every species that we've studied to date, sleep or something that looks very much like
00:42:42.100
it seems to exist. And what that has suggested is that sleep evolved with life itself on this planet
00:42:51.060
and has fought its way through heroically every step along the evolutionary pathway.
00:42:56.980
Let's linger on that point. That's very interesting because you can imagine the adaptive benefits that
00:43:03.960
would generally accrue to any species that could just get over its need for sleep. I mean,
00:43:10.860
there would have been, you would think, a selective pressure in the direction of completely erasing
00:43:16.020
sleep. So it suggests that it's rather hard to do.
00:43:19.740
I think it's a beautiful way of thinking about it because from an evolutionary perspective,
00:43:25.280
just as you noted, it is the most idiotic of all things. Firstly, when you're asleep, you're not
00:43:30.540
eating, you're not foraging for food, you're not finding a mate, you're not reproducing, you're not
00:43:35.600
caring for your young. And worst of all, as you noted, you're vulnerable to predation. So on any one
00:43:41.140
of those grounds, but especially all of them as a collective, sleep should have been strongly
00:43:46.920
selected against during the course of evolution. And it's once been said that if sleep doesn't
00:43:53.840
serve an absolutely vital function, then it's the biggest mistake the evolutionary process ever made.
00:43:59.280
And what we've now since learned is that Mother Nature didn't make a spectacular blunder in creating
00:44:04.260
this thing called sleep. But even very old evolutionary, you know, species like earthworms,
00:44:11.520
for example, seem to have periods of, it's called lethargic, or essentially a sleep-like state.
00:44:18.940
You know, this takes sleep back millions of years. Even some bacteria that seem to live at least
00:44:25.220
several days, they will have an active phase and a passive phase, perhaps the precursor to sleep.
00:44:32.280
So you're right, you could well imagine why, if some species had understood a way to circumnavigate
00:44:41.260
its way around the essential need for sleep, it would have dominated for lots of different reasons,
00:44:46.880
at least within its species category. The fact that we haven't seen that yet argues that sleep must
00:44:53.600
be fundamental at the most basic of biological levels. And it's one of the reasons why when people
00:44:59.880
will say to me, well, look, can't you, you know, if you're a doctor training, I think we learned to
00:45:06.640
overcome our need for sleep. We learned to tolerate and deal with insufficient sleep, and you can do
00:45:13.220
that. If you could, trust me, I think Mother, you know, there's some degree of hubris there, which is
00:45:18.820
Mother Nature, if she could have even halved the amount of time that you are vulnerable to all of
00:45:25.860
those vicissitudes of sleep. She certainly would have. And the fact that it's been preserved tells
00:45:34.200
you that doesn't seem to be possible. And within the lifespan, we think that we can come along and
00:45:38.940
within a 10-year training of a career, we could overcome it. It's unlikely to be the case.
00:45:44.480
Actually, we might punctuate this part of the conversation with the cases of various people
00:45:51.460
who, at least by their own testimony, have gone a fair way toward overcoming their personal need
00:45:59.760
for sleep. I think it was Winston Churchill who, during the war years, was sleeping the last 10
00:46:05.960
minutes of every hour or something like that. I don't know if that's apocryphal, but what do we
00:46:11.340
know about anyone's successfully titrating their sleep down to something like a minimum? I'm sure
00:46:21.340
there are genotypes here that we may know something about where people just require less sleep than
00:46:26.360
is normal. But actually, I once had a doctor who claimed to sleep no more than three and a half hours
00:46:34.920
a night. And, you know, whether he was, again, this is before the age of sleep tracking, so he could
00:46:42.320
have been delusional. But what do we know about people who sleep much less than you would recommend?
00:46:50.220
Firstly, from an epidemiological or population-based perspective, which is simply associational,
00:46:55.240
using that sweet spot that we recommend, which is somewhere between seven to nine hours a night for
00:46:59.960
the average adult, once you start to get less than that, the shorter your sleep, the shorter your
00:47:05.380
life. That short sleep predicts or cause mortality. Are there people in history who have claimed to be
00:47:12.620
short sleepers? There are. And Churchill was one. Edison was another, although Edison was a habitual
00:47:19.200
napper during the day, and he used naps and sleep as a creative tool. Then you have Thatcher,
00:47:25.560
Margaret Thatcher, you have Ronald Reagan. Just named two people who ended their lives with
00:47:32.920
Alzheimer's. So that's not a great commercial for their strategies.
00:47:35.960
Exactly where I was going. Yeah. Yeah. You know, they seemed, if on the face of it,
00:47:41.700
to make it through until the, you know, 50s or even 60s. My goodness, there is evidential proof that you
00:47:49.040
can sleep what they claimed to be sleeping, which is four hours a night, and get away with it. And
00:47:54.860
ultimately, what we learned is that one way or another, sleep deficiency seems to get its hux into
00:48:02.940
you, that the elastic band of sleep deprivation can stretch only so far before it snaps. And
00:48:08.760
tragically, for both of those individuals, Thatcher and Reagan, they succumb to the disease of Alzheimer's.
00:48:17.080
And we now know that there are- I now realize we have several files open, but
00:48:20.300
each of these seems important. So on that point, how do we disentangle association and causation here?
00:48:29.380
Because couldn't it also be true that one of the early symptoms of Alzheimer's or, you know,
00:48:34.860
being at a special risk for it is to have one's apparent ability to sleep diminish over the course
00:48:41.880
of one's life, even maybe starting as early as one's 30s or 40s?
00:48:46.780
Yeah, so we can go, Alzheimer's disease is actually a great example. It's probably been,
00:48:52.000
I think, one of the most exciting areas of sleep research in terms of discoveries in the past 10
00:48:57.680
or even five years. We started with just those epidemiological associations, which are simply
00:49:03.260
that, the correlation, the not causation. And what that told us is that people who were
00:49:07.900
if you'd like to continue listening to this conversation, you'll need to subscribe at
00:49:16.020
samharris.org. Once you do, you'll get access to all full-length episodes of the Making Sense
00:49:20.680
podcast, along with other subscriber-only content, including bonus episodes and AMAs and the
00:49:26.960
conversations I've been having on the Waking Up app. The Making Sense podcast is ad-free and relies
00:49:32.260
entirely on listener support, and you can subscribe now at samharris.org.