Making Sense - Sam Harris - August 09, 2017


#91 — The Biology of Good and Evil


Episode Stats

Length

42 minutes

Words per Minute

157.85187

Word Count

6,782

Sentence Count

354

Misogynist Sentences

1

Hate Speech Sentences

19


Summary

Robert Sapolsky is a neuroendocrinologist and primatologist at Stanford University, and the recipient of a MacArthur Genius Grant. In this episode, we discuss his new book, Behave: The Biology of Humans at Our Best and Worst, which is the most accessible discussion of brain science you will find. And for those of you who want more talk about free will, about the fact that the concept doesn t make much sense, and about why that matters, we get into that at the end. We also talk about how he got into neuroscience, and why he thinks primatology and neurobiology are such a good combination. And, of course, we talk about why you should read Behave before you get into the details of the brain, because it's the best book you'll find about the brain you can read. If you like what you hear here, please consider becoming a supporter of the podcast by becoming a subscriber. We don't run ads on the podcast, and therefore, therefore, we are made possible entirely through the support of our subscribers, who support what we're doing here, through becoming a member of the Making Sense Club. Thanks to all of our supporters, we can't do this without you! Thank you to Sam Harris for being a supporter, and for supporting the podcast. You're making sense of this podcast, making sense. Sam Harris - making sense and making sense, of it all. - and we're making it all of us better, so we can all be better. Thank you, and more of you, too, and we can be more like you. . Thanks, Sam Harris, too! - Your support is so much appreciated, and you'll be helping us make sense, more of this, and it helps us all get a better of it. and we'll all have a better listening experience, too much more of it, and that helps us to make sense of the world. -- thank you, more listening to the podcast -- more of that, and better listening to us, and a better podcast, more sense of us all, and so much more! -- and more listening, and less of us, more understanding of the things we can help us all can do more of us making sense in the world, and they'll be better listening of it -- thanks, you'll get a chance to help us, too.


Transcript

00:00:00.000 Welcome to the Making Sense Podcast.
00:00:08.820 This is Sam Harris.
00:00:10.880 Just a note to say that if you're hearing this, you are not currently on our subscriber
00:00:14.680 feed and will only be hearing the first part of this conversation.
00:00:18.420 In order to access full episodes of the Making Sense Podcast, you'll need to subscribe at
00:00:22.720 samharris.org.
00:00:24.060 There you'll find our private RSS feed to add to your favorite podcatcher, along with
00:00:28.360 other subscriber-only content.
00:00:30.520 We don't run ads on the podcast, and therefore it's made possible entirely through the support
00:00:34.640 of our subscribers.
00:00:35.880 So if you enjoy what we're doing here, please consider becoming one.
00:00:46.440 Today I'm speaking with Robert Sapolsky.
00:00:49.460 Robert is a neuroendocrinologist and a primatologist.
00:00:53.420 He's a professor of biology and neurology at Stanford University.
00:00:56.800 And the recipient of a MacArthur so-called genius grant.
00:01:02.440 I don't know if that's the official title of that grant.
00:01:04.980 Does one have to say so-called there?
00:01:07.700 In any case, Robert really is a superstar professor and communicator of science, as well as a top
00:01:13.760 flight scientist.
00:01:15.620 And as I say in the beginning, I remember being in a class at Stanford when he came in as a
00:01:21.080 guest lecturer, and I recall that being one of the moments that nudged me toward doing my PhD in
00:01:27.100 neuroscience, rather than philosophy.
00:01:29.360 I've been wanting to speak to Robert for quite some time.
00:01:32.720 He's actually been one of my most frequently requested guests.
00:01:36.060 In this episode, we discuss his new book, Behave, the biology of humans at our best and worst,
00:01:42.720 which I highly recommend.
00:01:44.320 It really is the most accessible discussion of brain science you will find.
00:01:50.700 And for those of you who want more talk about free will, about the fact that the concept
00:01:56.240 doesn't make much sense, and about why that matters, we get into that at the end.
00:02:03.160 And now, without further delay, I bring you Robert Sapolsky.
00:02:06.580 I am here with Robert Sapolsky.
00:02:14.620 Robert, thanks for coming on the podcast.
00:02:17.060 Sure.
00:02:17.480 Glad to be here.
00:02:18.960 As you and I know, but our listeners don't, we have been fighting our robot overlords to
00:02:24.220 get a clear connection here.
00:02:25.900 Now, with two attempts, and I think we've got it, but as I've said, if this glitches on us,
00:02:31.760 I will get on a plane and come and interview you, because we have a lot to talk about.
00:02:35.520 So, welcome to the podcast.
00:02:37.480 Thanks for persisting here.
00:02:39.500 I'm trying to check my memory here.
00:02:41.340 I don't think you and I have ever met.
00:02:43.200 We're in very much similar circles, but I recall, actually, when I was at Stanford, I was in a
00:02:49.600 class with John Gabrielli, I think on the neuroanatomy of memory, and you were brought in as a guest
00:02:56.160 lecturer, and so that was my first exposure to you, and really fairly early exposure to what
00:03:02.680 is interesting about brain science.
00:03:04.780 You gave this very cool interdisciplinary talk, because you are both a neurobiologist and a
00:03:11.340 primatologist, and you should know that you, standing in front of a class of undergraduates
00:03:17.560 for the better part of an hour, got into my brain and inspired me, in part, to go the direction
00:03:23.900 I did.
00:03:24.380 So, thank you for that.
00:03:25.360 Well, thanks.
00:03:26.000 That's really good to hear.
00:03:28.780 So, you've written this book, Behave, which is just this monumental tour of the human brain
00:03:35.420 and behavior, and we will cover some of it.
00:03:38.260 We definitely will not exhaust what is interesting in that book.
00:03:42.960 But what you did here is, as I know all too well, it's really hard to write about the brain
00:03:47.960 in a way that's accessible, because it's not so much that the concepts are so hard, but
00:03:53.620 once you get into the details and you start naming parts, it just becomes this thicket of
00:03:59.440 neuroanatomical terms, and people totally lose the plot.
00:04:03.240 You really do a fantastic job in this book of giving scientific detail in a way that is
00:04:08.740 not at all boring and really quite accessible.
00:04:12.560 Honestly, this is not something I have managed to do in my books, and that's why when I bring
00:04:16.420 the relevant neuroscience, I kind of get in and get out as quickly as possible, because
00:04:21.200 it makes for brutal reading, but you've struck a wonderful balance here.
00:04:25.400 So, more praise to you.
00:04:27.560 Well, thanks.
00:04:28.460 I have had to survive neuroanatomy classes, so I know exactly how awful all the multi-syllabic
00:04:35.200 names can be.
00:04:36.600 So, I'm still traumatized myself.
00:04:39.420 Yeah, yeah.
00:04:40.160 So, I want to talk from, before we get into your book, just about your background here
00:04:47.140 and the way in which you've married what is essentially neuroendocrinology and primatology,
00:04:53.920 which is a fairly unique combination.
00:04:56.940 I can't imagine there are too many of you at meetings with the same bio.
00:05:00.540 So, how has primatology informed your study of the brain, and if I'm not mistaken, you
00:05:07.320 focus exclusively on baboons, right?
00:05:09.540 So, how has the picking of baboons been relevant here?
00:05:13.900 Well, sort of the common theme in my work has been to understand the effects of stress
00:05:20.240 on health, in particular, the effects of stress on the brain.
00:05:23.660 And what do you know, the punchline for all of that is stress can do some pretty lousy things
00:05:28.980 to the brain.
00:05:30.180 What I've spent many decades doing is, as you say, sort of oscillating between being
00:05:37.080 a lab scientist, growing neurons in petri dishes, mucking around with their genes and
00:05:42.900 such, and then for 32 summers, picking up and going to a national park in East Africa and
00:05:51.120 studying baboons there.
00:05:52.400 Um, and these are the same animals I return to each year, um, animals I can dart, can anesthetize,
00:06:00.640 and when they're unconscious, do a whole bunch of, like, basic sort of clinical tests you
00:06:06.020 would do in a human.
00:06:07.280 Um, in terms of balancing the two, they've always kind of complemented each other in that
00:06:13.100 you observe something or other interesting about the brain based on your petri dish neurons
00:06:19.540 or your lab rats.
00:06:20.960 And that's great.
00:06:22.120 But the question, of course, is whether this, like, actually tells us anything about the
00:06:26.280 real world.
00:06:27.100 Let's go study a primate in its natural habitat.
00:06:30.140 And then you see something interesting behaviorally, uh, with these wild primates.
00:06:35.320 And you say, geez, I wonder, like, if it's this part of the brain or what's going on there.
00:06:41.100 And thus you go back to the lab and you're cultured neurons.
00:06:45.480 So it's, it's been sort of, uh, a very privileged, uh, ability to do these sort of complementary
00:06:52.620 approaches.
00:06:53.940 Hmm.
00:06:54.380 I'm now picturing you darting these baboons.
00:06:57.300 Do you do it yourself?
00:06:58.300 Do you actually fire the gun?
00:07:00.220 Um, it's a blowgun.
00:07:01.820 Um, it takes surprisingly little practice.
00:07:06.460 Um, fortunately baboons have very large rear ends, which is what you aim for.
00:07:10.880 Um, and you know, um, you're, um, I'm beyond cliched is like, you're nice liberal.
00:07:17.740 And so the ability to like sort of sneak around the bush with a blowgun and shoot it while baboons
00:07:23.220 and like do that, it's great.
00:07:25.660 It's like, and, and you're doing conservation work the whole time.
00:07:29.460 So it's, uh, yeah, it's a blast.
00:07:31.980 I love doing it.
00:07:33.480 Now, do the baboons recognize you enough to, and recognize what you're doing enough for
00:07:39.200 the consequences of your darting to form a grievance against you darting them?
00:07:43.880 Um, not if things go well, I mean, sort of 90% of the time out there, uh, you're collecting
00:07:50.320 behavioral data, which is your basic Jane Goodall sort of scene where you're just hanging with
00:07:55.680 them from, from dawn till dusk.
00:07:58.200 And there's sort of a whole science about doing it in a quantitative objective kind of
00:08:02.900 way.
00:08:03.660 Um, so it's actually an infrequent day where I'm out darting.
00:08:07.520 But one of the things, um, that actually makes it quite difficult is you can't dart somebody
00:08:13.560 until there's nobody else around and nobody looking.
00:08:17.640 And he's turned the other way, you dart him and he responds as if he's been stung by a
00:08:24.140 bee or has sat on a thorn, uh, jumps up, scratches his rear for a second, sits back down.
00:08:30.020 And then three minutes later, he's unconscious.
00:08:32.340 So you get one alone and, and when he's unconscious, you can approach him and, and no one else intervenes
00:08:40.740 from the troop or, or notices what you're doing at that point.
00:08:43.640 Well, that's when it all goes perfectly smoothly when it doesn't, he decides to pick up in
00:08:49.560 those three minutes before going under and walk over and sit down right in the middle
00:08:54.620 of a gazillion other baboons or get into a fight with somebody.
00:08:58.420 And those are the, those are the ones that don't go so well.
00:09:03.080 What are you doing to the reputations of these baboons that walk among their troop and start
00:09:08.060 a fight and then promptly faint from your anesthesia?
00:09:10.640 Well, it's, um, it's got to cause all sorts of interesting belief systems in these animals
00:09:18.460 that I can't quite access.
00:09:20.520 Oh, that's funny.
00:09:22.060 Now, are there disanalogies between baboons and humans that are of interest here?
00:09:27.940 Because they're further from us than chimps.
00:09:30.400 So are there, are there ways in which chimps are similar to humans and, and baboons aren't?
00:09:36.200 And if so, why the choice to study baboons?
00:09:39.660 Um, chimps would be much better insofar as, you know, the cliche, uh, they share and do
00:09:47.020 share 98% of our genome with us, uh, far closer in terms of social structure, in terms of cognitive,
00:09:54.060 emotional capacities, all of that.
00:09:55.920 Nonetheless, uh, baboons still count as close relatives.
00:09:59.180 And I think we've 96% share DNA for a bunch of reasons.
00:10:05.320 Baboons are perfect for what I do.
00:10:07.340 Um, they live out in the open and these big open grasslands.
00:10:10.700 So you can see them 12 hours a day and you can actually like see them to dart them.
00:10:15.280 They, they don't live up in trees.
00:10:17.180 They're not endangered.
00:10:18.460 They're, they're big.
00:10:19.760 They've got lots of blood that you could borrow from them for your, uh, sort of tests.
00:10:24.800 Um, but probably most of all, given that I study stress, none of us are getting stressed
00:10:31.520 because we're like riddled with, with, you know, diphtheria or some horrible chronic illness.
00:10:38.380 None of us are getting stressed because we're chased by saber teeth tigers every day.
00:10:42.680 Instead we're Westernized humans, which is to say we get chronic psychosocial stress.
00:10:48.800 And it turns out baboons are absolutely perfect for this.
00:10:53.520 They live in these large troops, 50 to a hundred animals.
00:10:58.080 Um, the Serengeti where they live is like a perfect ecosystem.
00:11:02.360 Predators don't hassle them much and they only have to work about three hours a day for their
00:11:07.080 calories.
00:11:07.820 And what that means is you've got nine hours of free time every day to devote to making
00:11:14.180 some other baboon miserable.
00:11:16.420 All, all they do is generate social stress for each other.
00:11:19.940 They're perfect models for Westernized humans.
00:11:22.260 Now, what about mandrills?
00:11:24.580 They look like baboons, but they're not baboons, right?
00:11:27.860 Am I, am I right about that?
00:11:28.740 Oh, there's some major taxonomic civil war going on about that as to whether they're,
00:11:33.860 they are a baboon type.
00:11:36.080 I've steered clear of that one because I'm, I'm fairly, uh, uncommitted to it, but they're
00:11:42.200 different social system.
00:11:44.700 They live in dense rainforest.
00:11:45.840 They would be mighty hard to study.
00:11:48.600 These savannah baboons that I focus on are, are perfect.
00:11:52.400 So now get into your book, which is really this wonderful tour of the brain and behavior
00:12:00.780 and, you know, morally salient behavior.
00:12:03.260 You and I approach these questions from a pretty similar angle and we agree about many
00:12:09.060 things.
00:12:09.400 I'm sure we'll talk about free will at one point because many of our listeners want us
00:12:13.740 to, and you are one of the few people who have made more or less the same noises on this
00:12:18.640 topic that I have in science.
00:12:20.580 We've, we've, we've broken the same taboo here, but that'll be fun to talk about.
00:12:24.140 But to start with where you're, you're coming from, you have a kind of unity of knowledge
00:12:30.260 approach and, and you, you look at the various levels of scientific explanation from neurophysiological
00:12:38.300 and genetic to psychological and cultural.
00:12:42.120 And each of these clearly has a different language game associated with it.
00:12:47.600 But you like, like I don't make much of the transitions between these levels, but you do
00:12:54.520 something interesting here where you, you find a novel way of segmenting these different
00:12:58.560 levels of analysis with respect to time, the, the, the prox, the proximity to causing
00:13:04.520 human behavior, which is very interesting.
00:13:07.100 So talk about how you break down the levels of scientific explanation in a temporal sense.
00:13:14.100 Like as behavioral biologists, which most of us are in some stripe or other, um, a behavior
00:13:20.340 occurs and we are in a sense asking why did that behavior just happen?
00:13:25.320 And it turns out that's actually asking a whole bunch of questions, because if you're
00:13:30.420 asking, why did that behavior just happen?
00:13:33.780 Part of it is what occurred in the brain of that individual one second ago.
00:13:39.320 But you're also saying what were the sensory cues in the environment a minute ago that triggered
00:13:46.480 those neurons?
00:13:47.180 And you're also asking what did that person's hormone levels this morning over the recent
00:13:53.540 hours or days have to do with making them more or less sensitive to those sensory cues,
00:13:59.120 which then trigger those neurons.
00:14:01.120 And then you're often running to neuroplasticity over the course of months, back to childhood,
00:14:07.680 back to fetal environment, which turns out to be phenomenally influential on adult behavior.
00:14:14.320 And then you're back to genes, but then if you're still asking why did that behavior occur?
00:14:19.020 You're also asking, well, what sort of culture was this person raised in?
00:14:24.040 Which often winds up meaning what were this person's ancestors doing a couple of hundred
00:14:28.020 years ago?
00:14:29.220 What were the ecological influences on that?
00:14:31.760 And finally, when you're saying why did that behavior occur?
00:14:35.440 You're also asking something about the millions of years of evolutionary pressures beforehand.
00:14:40.820 So it's not just the case that, ooh, it's important to remember to look at these things at multiple
00:14:47.740 levels.
00:14:48.740 Exactly as you said, ultimately they merge into the same.
00:14:52.260 If you're talking about the brain, you're talking about the childhood experiences when
00:14:57.440 the brain was assembled.
00:14:59.020 If you're talking about genes, you're implicitly talking about the evolution of them.
00:15:04.160 All of these just are a confluence of influences on behavior that are all sort of interconnected.
00:15:12.080 Yeah.
00:15:13.080 Yeah.
00:15:14.080 Well, we'll get back to that, precisely that picture when we talk about free will because
00:15:18.980 obviously there's a lot of confusion about degrees of freedom for the mind when you're
00:15:25.640 talking about the neurophysiology of human behavior or the way in which culture influences brain
00:15:32.120 development.
00:15:33.120 The punchline here, obviously, is that once you grant that the brain is the final common
00:15:39.240 pathway of all these influences when you're talking about human thought and intention and
00:15:43.520 behavior, well then you have to grant that what the brain is doing is the proximate cause of what the person is doing.
00:15:51.560 And either you're going to sign on to the laws of physics here or you're not, so we'll get back to that.
00:15:59.240 But there's a common misunderstanding around the relationship between reason and emotion, just across the board, but in particular with respect to human behavior and the answering of moral questions,
00:16:14.520 the way in which we just form a worldview that is this idea that you can be emotionally motivated
00:16:21.160 or you can be motivated by emotion-free rationality.
00:16:26.520 Let's perform a little psychosurgery on that idea.
00:16:30.120 You treat this in your book.
00:16:31.940 How do you think about reason and emotion?
00:16:33.920 Well, it's the inevitable like Coke versus Pepsi dichotomy there.
00:16:39.240 And as to which is more important, which influences the other more in terms of our actions, and of course, it turns out, as with most sort of dichotomies with behavior, it's a false one.
00:16:51.960 They're utterly intertwined and intertwined on a neurobiological level.
00:16:57.960 You have a thought, you think of something terrifying that happened to you long ago and emotional parts of your brain activate and you secrete stress hormones.
00:17:08.280 Or you have an aroused emotion, you're in an agitated, frightened state, and suddenly you think and reason in a way that's like imprudent and ridiculous.
00:17:19.880 We make terrible decisions often.
00:17:22.060 When we're aroused, the two parts are equally intertwined.
00:17:25.080 But probably where the most progress in thinking about this intertwining has come in recent years is, you know, there's a certain sort of comfort and I think chauvinism we take as these like creatures with big cortexes and thinking that nonetheless reason is sort of at the core of most of our decision making.
00:17:45.660 And an awful lot of work has shown that far more often than we would like to think, we make our decisions based on implicit emotional automatic reflexes.
00:17:58.180 We make them within milliseconds.
00:18:00.820 Parts of the brain that are marinated in emotion and hormones are activating from the standpoint of the brain long, long before the more cortical rational parts activate.
00:18:12.100 And often what we believe is rational thinking is instead our cognitive selves playing catch up to try to rationalize why our emotional instincts actually are perfectly logical and make wonderful sense.
00:18:26.180 And in lots of ways, the best way to show this is you manipulate the affective, the emotional, the automatic, the implicit, the subterranean aspects of our brains where we may not even be aware of it.
00:18:43.400 And it changes our decisions and then we come up with highfalutin explanations for why it's actually because of some philosopher we read freshman year.
00:18:52.980 That's why I did what I just did.
00:18:55.000 No, actually, it's because of this manipulation that just occurred.
00:18:58.740 Yeah, yeah.
00:18:59.120 So there are really two sides of this.
00:19:00.780 So that's one side which is kind of deflationary of cognition and reason.
00:19:06.160 So you think your reasoning and that your reasoning is driving your cognition or your belief formation.
00:19:12.800 But then when you look closely, you find that it's being driven from below by emotion.
00:19:18.040 But the flip side of that is that in order to make even the most coolly calculated reasoning effective, it needs to be integrated with parts of the brain.
00:19:28.680 In this case, the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, that you need a felt sense of the consequences of being right or wrong.
00:19:35.580 And this connects to Damasio's work and others where people who have neurological damage there, they may know the correct strategy, say, in a gambling task.
00:19:45.880 They may understand the probabilities, but they can't make that understanding effective because it doesn't actually mean anything.
00:19:54.940 It's not coded appropriately.
00:19:56.440 Yep.
00:19:57.440 And it turns out this is like a tremendous rebuke for the people out there who would say, if only we could be purely rational creatures, if only we can get rid of all that affective muck from underneath, why we'd all be Mr. Spock and it would be a wondrous world.
00:20:14.200 And exactly as you say, ventromedial prefrontal cortex work of people like Damasio, people who have damage to this part of the brain that's basically the means by which your emotional parts of the brain talk to your most rational ones and tell them what they're feeling, get damaged there.
00:20:33.080 And people make decisions about things that we view as appalling, as beyond the pale of cold-blooded, as detached.
00:20:42.500 As one example, you take any normal person on Earth and you give them sort of a philosophy problem.
00:20:50.560 Would you kill one stranger to save the life of five?
00:20:53.980 And they maybe say yes, maybe say no.
00:20:57.380 And then you say, would you kill your parent, your loved one, to save the life of five?
00:21:03.160 And in half a second, you say, no, of course not.
00:21:06.480 It's my mother.
00:21:07.660 It's my child, whatever.
00:21:09.140 And you take somebody in damage to this part of the brain and they give the exact same answer.
00:21:15.900 It doesn't register.
00:21:17.900 They don't process relatedness in the same way.
00:21:20.900 And every primate on Earth would look at that and say there's something desperately wrong with this person's brain.
00:21:28.080 On this issue of emotion and rationality, one point I have begun making, which I haven't heard made, and I just want to bounce it off of you.
00:21:37.460 So I've begun to think about doubt, which really is one of the core foundations of our rationality, right?
00:21:45.420 I mean, so you say something which I find implausible, kind of my error detection mechanism, whether it's logical or factual or semantic or based on memory.
00:21:56.700 You know, something gets tripped.
00:21:57.800 You utter a sentence and I don't buy it.
00:22:01.160 That feeling of doubt, in my view, really is an emotion.
00:22:05.200 And, you know, we actually have some neuroimaging data to back this up in that all the fMRI studies I did on belief showed that disbelief, doubting the veracity of a proposition, was associated with activity in the anterior insula.
00:22:20.460 And I've actually begun to think of doubt as a kind of emotion on the continuum of disgust, as kind of propositional disgust or cognitive disgust.
00:22:31.600 And, you know, frankly, when I see our president speak, I find I'm viscerally in touch with doubt as disgust, right?
00:22:38.980 I mean, like there's a certain level of incredulity in the face of, you know, a confident utterance that precipitates in me, at least, a fairly strong emotion of disgust.
00:22:49.400 So I just I want to put those data in front of you and just get your take.
00:22:53.560 Well, my my insular cortex is right with you on that one.
00:22:57.300 But I I thoroughly agree with it.
00:23:01.080 Obviously, there's some domains where doubt is just a purely rational process.
00:23:06.620 You sit there and you add up two and two and somehow it comes out to five.
00:23:12.000 And that's a fairly pure cognitive state of saying, I doubt if that actually is correct.
00:23:18.080 But most of the doubts we have and in our social world, I think you're absolutely right, is steeped in emotion, emotion, disgust, perhaps at the person who is sowing that doubt, emotion.
00:23:33.220 Robert, just to clarify, if we put you in a scanner and give you propositions just like that, two plus two equals five, you're six foot five inches tall, you're a woman with blonde hair, George Washington was never president of the United States.
00:23:48.940 I just give you propositional statements which you recognize to be untrue, but which are not, in fact, emotionally laden.
00:23:57.480 I would certainly predict on the basis of now three neuroimaging studies that those would be associated with insular activity in you and the same statements in a positive light that you would accept.
00:24:11.820 You know, George Washington was the first president of the United States wouldn't.
00:24:14.720 I completely agree because it's very much context dependent.
00:24:19.460 If I were sitting there on my own and adding up two and two and I got five, I would have, you know, a half seconds worth of pure rationality.
00:24:28.780 Wait a second, that's not right.
00:24:30.800 And then I would have, that's it.
00:24:33.060 I'm an idiot.
00:24:33.920 I'm a fake.
00:24:35.120 Everybody else is going to finally figure it out.
00:24:36.940 I can't even add two plus two.
00:24:38.440 And sitting there in the brain imager, I think you're absolutely right.
00:24:42.560 That would not be a purely cognitive experience.
00:24:45.300 That would be, what are these guys up to?
00:24:48.220 Do I trust them?
00:24:49.320 Do I feel safe here?
00:24:50.580 Do they think I'm an idiot?
00:24:52.060 What do they think of me?
00:24:53.080 Did I say something foolish before?
00:24:54.800 Are they going to lock me up in the scanner, et cetera?
00:24:57.340 And often running with emotional aspects.
00:24:59.960 I think one of the most perfect realms for looking at this is when you look at conformity studies and where people go along with something that is patently untrue, yet they go along.
00:25:13.880 A certain percentage of them are just being affable.
00:25:16.500 They're being publicly conforming.
00:25:19.000 But a certain percentage actually change their minds.
00:25:22.880 And you can see activation of the visual cortex.
00:25:26.880 Hey, remember, you actually saw something different than you're saying.
00:25:31.260 You saw what all of them are saying.
00:25:32.880 This is a state that's also associated with activation of the insular cortex, activation of the amygdala.
00:25:40.200 It's anxiety.
00:25:41.680 Doubting provokes anxiety.
00:25:44.460 Certainty is a very comforting thing.
00:25:46.480 And doubting even seemingly the most cerebral and sort of soulless of issues out there, nonetheless readily taps into all these senses of anxiety running underneath there.
00:26:01.660 There's this other piece here, which is that the brain doesn't have, I mean, this is just a constraint of evolution.
00:26:08.480 We were not built so as to acquire new cognitive abilities de novo.
00:26:17.040 The only material to use for modern human cognition are these ancient structures that have to be commandeered to new purposes.
00:26:26.780 So everything we do is built on the back of these apish structures.
00:26:33.920 You know, here we're talking about the insula, which does receive the inputs from the viscera.
00:26:40.020 You know, you find rotting food disgusting.
00:26:43.000 You know, that is the tale told by the insula.
00:26:45.200 And the only way to build a mind that has the capacity to find abstract ideas repugnant is to be repurposing or extending the purpose of these brain areas that were doing nothing of the kind in apes like ourselves that couldn't form abstract ideas.
00:27:06.720 Absolutely. And it's a totally fascinating domain, the fact that this insular cortex, which if you're like a mole, will tell you if you're eating something rotten, activates in humans thinking about moral disgust.
00:27:22.140 That a part of the brain that does temperature sensing for you is also activated when you're contemplating whether somebody has a warm or cold personality, that the parts of your brain, some parts that are involved in pain detection, in a very literal sense, also activate when you're feeling empathic about somebody else's pain.
00:27:42.600 And all of these speak to this sort of altruism about evolution. Evolution is not an inventor. It's a tinkerer. It makes do with what's already there.
00:27:54.280 I don't know. When did humans come up with the concept of moral outrage and moral disgust? Maybe in the last 20,000 years, 50,000, whatever.
00:28:02.800 When did we come up with the concept of having warm or cold personalities? A lot shorter than that.
00:28:08.080 And at that point, nobody sits down and says, okay, we need to evolve an entirely new part of the brain that does moral disgust.
00:28:17.060 They say, insula. I know. That kind of sounds the same. They do food disgust.
00:28:23.040 Like, here, give me some duct tape. We're just going to push that into the insula.
00:28:27.040 And now the insula also does metaphorical moral disgust.
00:28:30.440 And it's a brain that's winging it in a lot of ways for some really interesting ways in which it's better and ways in which that's for the worse.
00:28:41.000 When you think about the role of the brain in producing these kinds of purely human-level distinctions, things like the birth of civilization, really,
00:28:51.900 I mean, it's largely a story of what the frontal cortex is doing.
00:28:56.680 I think you say at one point in the book that this region of the brain is what makes you do the hard thing when the hard thing is the right thing to do.
00:29:04.820 Let's talk for a moment about the role of the frontal cortex in our species.
00:29:09.900 That's exactly sort of a summary of what it does.
00:29:14.180 More jargony, it does impulse control and emotional regulation and long-term planning and gratification postponement and executive function.
00:29:24.900 It's the part of the brain that attempts to tell you, you know, this seems like a good idea right now, but trust me, you're going to regret it.
00:29:32.660 Don't do it. Don't do it.
00:29:33.780 Of great importance, it's the most recently evolved part of our brains.
00:29:38.800 Our frontal cortex is proportionately bigger and or more complex than in any other primate.
00:29:45.860 And most interestingly, it's the last part of the brain to get fully wired up.
00:29:52.100 I mean, we're accustomed to images of, you know, your brain is pretty much set to go by the time you're at kindergarten.
00:29:57.900 The frontal cortex is not fully online until people are on the average about a quarter century old, which is boggling, which is boggling.
00:30:08.940 But it also tells you a whole lot about why adolescents act in adolescent ways because the frontal cortex isn't very powerful yet.
00:30:16.860 But in that is an incredibly interesting implication, which is if the frontal cortex that does all this complex, like culture-specific reasoning and regulating your behavior,
00:30:32.840 if the frontal cortex is the last part of the brain to fully mature, by definition, it's the part of the brain that is least constrained by genes and most shaped by experience.
00:30:44.580 And that's real important.
00:30:47.880 Because think about, I mean, sort of, okay, the frontal cortex, it's your moral barometer, if that's the right metaphor, it's your Calvinist voice whispering in your head.
00:30:59.160 So the frontal cortex, for example, plays a central role if you're tempted to lie about something.
00:31:05.560 And if you manage to avoid that temptation, your frontal cortex had something to do with it.
00:31:10.740 But at the same time, if you do decide to lie, your frontal cortex plays an enormous role in you doing an effective job at lying, because that's a version of frontal regulation also.
00:31:22.800 Okay, control my voice.
00:31:25.000 Don't make eye contact.
00:31:26.500 Don't let my face do something funny.
00:31:28.200 That's a frontal task also.
00:31:29.900 So, you know, if you're talking about a part of the brain that is both central to you avoiding lying, but once you've decided to lie, is central to you doing it effectively, this is a very human, very complicated part of our brains.
00:31:44.880 Yeah, and so to follow on what you just said there about the implications of it being so late to develop, this is really where it matters what culture you're in and what early life experience you have.
00:31:58.820 What kind of person you become with respect to your beliefs about ethical norms and, you know, what constitutes honor and everything that stands out as a salient, consequential difference between groups and societies.
00:32:16.020 Now, none of this is just floating around in the ether, it's not just in the books on our shelves, this is getting etched in the brains of all concerned, and largely this is a story of what is happening in the frontal cortex.
00:32:32.340 Yep, and that's exactly why it can't mature until you're 25.
00:32:36.800 It's not that it's a more complex construction project than wiring up the rest of your brain is.
00:32:43.580 You need the first 25 years to learn your situational ethics and your culture-specific beliefs and that sort of thing, and those are subtle, and they're often unstated, and they're often exactly the opposite of what people tell you things are supposed to be about.
00:32:59.000 I mean, think about it, every culture on earth bans some types of killing and allows others, and they all do different ones.
00:33:08.540 Every culture on earth supports some types of lying and bans others.
00:33:13.320 In our culture, it's okay to lie to grandma to say, ooh, I don't have that toy, this is wonderful, thank you, thank you, when you've got the actual toy in your closet.
00:33:21.920 And it's okay for us to lie if somebody says, are you harboring those refugees in your attic, and you say, no, of course not, sir, of course not, SS officer.
00:33:32.380 You know, that one's okay, but there's other ones we've had.
00:33:34.760 Every culture has prohibitions about sexual behaviors, where some types of behaviors are wondrous and others are blasphemous, and they all differ, and that's a lot of subtle stuff to have to master as to what counts as doing the right thing.
00:33:49.980 Although I'm doing my part to not spare grandma the brutal truth about the toy.
00:33:55.100 That's a meme I'm trying to knock down.
00:33:57.140 I know, your book, Lying, certainly makes the most convincing sort of argument I've seen for it's not okay in any domain.
00:34:05.980 Yeah, although I think there's a misunderstanding there.
00:34:08.880 I think I saw in a footnote or an endnote, you said that I'm against lying in all conceivable circumstances, kind of the Kantian view, which is not the case.
00:34:18.360 I actually, if you're at the door with the Nazis and you've got Anne Frank in the attic, then I view lying the way most people do as an adequate and even necessary act of self-defense or the defense of others in those cases.
00:34:31.980 I really view it as being on a continuum of violence where it's the least violent thing you can do to someone who's no longer behaving as a rational actor or someone whose behavior you can modify with honest speech.
00:34:45.280 Okay, sorry for that misrepresentation.
00:34:48.620 Nobody was supposed to read the footnotes, so...
00:34:51.280 Well, Anne Frank is safe in my attic.
00:34:54.440 Oh, good.
00:34:54.760 So now, are there primates that show an analogous delay in maturation in the frontal cortex, or is that a uniquely human issue?
00:35:04.480 No, it's primate-wide.
00:35:06.440 It's even rodent-wide, but it's not as dramatic.
00:35:10.660 It's not as delayed.
00:35:11.680 It's not as faced with complex of a task as we do when learning sort of the complexities of our frontal-dependent prohibitions.
00:35:23.160 But no, we're not the only species that invented the idea that this is a very good part of the brain to make very malleable in the face of experience.
00:35:32.260 We've just got the most dramatic version.
00:35:36.020 There are other interesting bits of neuroanatomy here that you don't often hear talked about, at least in the popular press.
00:35:43.600 There's something called von Economo neurons, which are unique in primates and cetaceans and elephants, I believe.
00:35:51.240 And they're, I think, uniquely in the insula and anterior cingulate, or preferentially there, and they relate to social cognition and self-awareness.
00:36:01.100 Give us a potted description of what's happening there with these neurons.
00:36:05.980 They're very cool.
00:36:07.260 And you study human brains, and one of the first things you have to recognize is we're not humans because we've invented a totally novel type of neurotransmitter or a completely new brand of neuron.
00:36:18.620 It's just that we've got more of them.
00:36:20.560 They're more complexly wired.
00:36:22.680 But then people found this one neuron type that did seem to be unique to humans, these von Economos.
00:36:29.560 They're almost entirely found in anterior cingulate, insulate cortex, having to do with empathy and moral disgust and all that cool, interesting stuff.
00:36:41.000 So that's plenty interesting.
00:36:43.000 But as you say, then people looked further, and it turns up in other species and all the usual suspects when you're looking for the most complex social worlds.
00:36:52.840 Yeah, other apes, other primates, cetaceans, elephants.
00:36:57.660 And the best guess is that they play a role in some very complex aspects of sociality.
00:37:04.580 Are they mirror neurons?
00:37:06.940 And like, don't get me started on that one.
00:37:10.420 But very little reason to think they play the very narrowly documented sense of what these mirror neurons do.
00:37:17.680 That's a whole separate rant.
00:37:19.400 But one of the most interesting things is these are the first neurons that die in a very obscure neurological disease called frontotemporal dementia.
00:37:28.080 And one that predominantly damages the frontal cortex.
00:37:32.920 And the first neurons that go are von Economo neurons.
00:37:37.220 And two interesting things about that.
00:37:39.060 What that tells you is these are really expensive, vulnerable neurons to operate if they're the first ones that keel over.
00:37:46.760 But the other thing is, what does the disease look like?
00:37:50.600 Disinhibited, socially inappropriate behavior.
00:37:53.280 And often it's initially viewed as a psychiatric disorder until you realize this is a massive neurological sort of carpet bombing of the front part of your brain.
00:38:03.140 Whatever these neurons are doing, they are very, very much sort of specialized for the most complex social things we fancy species do.
00:38:13.260 Yeah, well, so I think the last stop on our Cook's tour of the brain here, or at least the Cook's tour of the frontal cortex, is the dorsolateral PFC, which is associated with much of what we consider to be higher rationality or executive control.
00:38:30.320 And activity here is able to dampen activity in emotional parts of the brain, like the limbic system, for instance, the amygdala in particular, reducing negative affect.
00:38:43.740 It can do this by becoming active in a relevant way.
00:38:48.420 And this is something that I think people understand, that if you cognitively reappraise what an experience means.
00:38:55.140 So, for instance, you know, you think someone's being rude to you, then you reconstrue that, realizing, say, that maybe he's just nervous.
00:39:03.320 And then that will dampen your initial negative emotional response to what you perceive to be rudeness.
00:39:10.660 But what's also interesting is that really any use of your dorsolateral prefrontal cortex can dampen negative arousal.
00:39:20.780 And if you just, if you're feeling negative emotion and you just put your attention on something else, you just start doing math problems,
00:39:27.800 or you do anything that requires an alternate form of cognition, that can have a similar effect of dampening arousal.
00:39:35.380 In thinking about this, it's always interesting to consider how someone like yourself, who spends a lot of time thinking about the mechanics of emotion and cognition from the brain side,
00:39:47.760 does this ever become relevant to you in your life, behaviorally?
00:39:52.360 I mean, do you ever, is there anything that you do differently in your moment-to-moment experience,
00:39:57.400 given how much time you spend thinking about what's going on under the hood?
00:40:01.580 For better or worse, yes.
00:40:06.460 And the same thing as infested my wife, who's a neuropsychologist by training.
00:40:12.360 Uh-oh.
00:40:12.800 Yeah, exactly.
00:40:13.920 When our kids were young, I remember this one day where our four-year-old son had just done something rotten to his two-year-old sister,
00:40:22.760 and we swooped in there, and we were doing the, you're not a bad person, but you did a bad thing,
00:40:28.160 and wailing on him with that one, and why did you do that?
00:40:31.620 And at some point, I don't even remember which of us said this, one of us would say,
00:40:36.700 why are we getting on him so hard here?
00:40:38.960 He has like three frontal neurons.
00:40:41.500 And the other one's response would be, well, how else is he going to develop a good frontal cortex?
00:40:47.320 So we actually, like, think that way in my house, which is pretty appalling when you think about it.
00:40:54.940 Although, sort of something I write about in the book, where I have the most trouble sort of applying my worldview as a mechanistic,
00:41:07.520 reductive, deterministic, sort of scientist guy thinking about behavior,
00:41:11.940 behavior is, as you say, when it's getting to the realm of free will.
00:41:16.200 Much like you, I don't believe there is free will.
00:41:19.400 I believe free will is what we call the biology that hasn't been discovered yet.
00:41:24.140 But what I find to be a hugely daunting task is how you're supposed to live your life thinking that way.
00:41:31.080 And even me with, like, I'm willing to write down and print, there's no free will, and here's why.
00:41:38.160 You know, at some critical juncture of some social interaction, I act absolutely as if I believe there's free will.
00:41:44.080 I hear about somebody who's done something jerky, and I wish horrible things to them,
00:41:49.260 instead of stopping and saying, oh, no, but think about what happened to them as a second trimester fetus.
00:41:55.000 You know, it's very hard to function with that.
00:41:58.640 I, like most people, I hit a wall with that one.
00:42:03.660 It's a whole lot easier to operate with the notion of agency.
00:42:07.900 Well, let's jump in there, because this is obviously a hugely consequential issue,
00:42:13.400 or maybe it's obvious to us.
00:42:15.100 It's not so obvious to most people, I think, that coming to a different conclusion about free will has consequences.
00:42:23.320 And, you know, I would argue they're quite good consequences, but let's get there.
00:42:28.680 So let's just step back and remind people of the problem.
00:42:32.700 If you'd like to continue listening to this conversation, you'll need to subscribe at samharris.org.
00:42:38.420 Once you do, you'll get access to all full-length episodes of the Making Sense podcast,
00:42:42.740 along with other subscriber-only content, including bonus episodes and AMAs,
00:42:47.760 and the conversations I've been having on the Waking Up app.
00:42:50.600 The Making Sense podcast is ad-free and relies entirely on listener support.
00:42:54.680 And you can subscribe now at samharris.org.