533. Dreams, Nightmares, and Neuroscience | Dr. Baland Jalal
Episode Stats
Length
1 hour and 45 minutes
Words per Minute
178.6717
Summary
Dr. Balan Jalal is a neuroscientist and author at Harvard and previously a visiting researcher at Cambridge University Medical School. He is one of the world s leading experts on sleep paralysis and the terrors and opportunities of dream, fantasy and adaptation that accompany that condition.
Transcript
00:00:00.000
So we look at the brain as something that's malleable and not fixed.
00:00:05.300
So the parietal lobes and the superior parietal lobulum
00:00:07.940
is specifically involved in creating a subjective sense of a self.
00:00:11.480
The feeling that I occupy this body and not somebody else's body, right?
00:00:14.900
Jung's idea was that the dream was a place of exploration
00:00:25.800
You're paralyzed from head to toe during REM sleep.
00:00:30.160
Something was happening to me and I was frozen and unable to speak
00:00:39.200
She'd have to come and shake me and then I'd wake up.
00:00:41.440
So I was sleeping in my room and then I had this creepy feeling
00:00:44.520
of a monster from the corner of my room approaching me
00:01:08.060
I had the opportunity today to speak to Dr. Balan Jalal.
00:01:12.940
He's a neuroscientist and author at Harvard and previously a visiting researcher
00:01:20.240
He's been featured in the New York Times, the Washington Post and other such publications.
00:01:25.280
The Telegraph and the BBC described him as one of the world's leading experts on sleep paralysis
00:01:30.660
and the terrors and opportunities of dream, fantasy, and adaptation that accompany that condition.
00:01:40.000
Dr. Jalal's Intro to Neuroscience course at Peterson Academy.
00:01:44.220
The relationship between neuroanatomy, brain function, perception, emotion, and behavior.
00:01:52.280
The role of culture and conception in dealing with emotion, particularly fear,
00:01:57.160
and the changing landscape of the modern university.
00:02:12.940
You just, we just released a course of yours, Introduction to Neuroscience.
00:02:17.000
So I'd like to know, I think it would be useful to talk a little bit about the course,
00:02:21.980
but I'd also like to know about your experience doing the filming and your,
00:02:27.160
reflections on the process and the project itself.
00:02:44.520
Ben, Nancy, Vincent, everybody on the team, lovely.
00:02:53.460
And then the course itself, it's an eight-hour course on the brain and sort of going through,
00:03:03.200
initially starting from sort of the basics of the brain, the different structures of
00:03:07.280
the brain, then going all the way to sort of higher abstract things like human nature
00:03:11.380
and sort of the nature of how art emanates in the brain and things like that.
00:03:17.120
So it's kind of has the nitty-gritty of an introductory neuroscience course, but then also
00:03:22.540
taking in some more sort of poetic aspects of the brain.
00:03:28.220
Right, so it's an approach to the cultural from the bottom up, from the biological up.
00:03:32.960
How much anatomy, functional anatomy and so forth is in the course?
00:03:39.160
So for a basics neuroscience course, as an introduction, you've got to have the basics there,
00:03:43.680
all the brain structures, the cortex, the brainstem, all the different names.
00:03:49.100
But I try to keep it simple so people don't fall asleep.
00:03:51.940
Well, I used to, when I did my first biopsychology neuroscience course, that was incredibly boring.
00:04:09.060
But I really made sure to describe the function and have an overall context for each.
00:04:15.620
Yeah, I found that I got extremely interested in neuroanatomy, even at a detailed level,
00:04:22.660
when I was reading scientists who associated the area with the function.
00:04:29.320
Yeah, well, and it was also extremely useful philosophically.
00:04:32.300
I mean, one of the advantages, if you're a conceptual thinker, to studying neuroanatomy and neurobiology is that it puts limits on what philosophical propositions are plausible and possible.
00:04:48.040
So it's like you have to play a game with two different sets of rules then, right?
00:04:55.580
It has to make sense conceptually, so that would be philosophically, but then it can't violate the principles of neuroanatomy and neuroscience that are already established.
00:05:08.840
Yeah, the way that I like to do is that I know that in this course I'll cover this, right?
00:05:13.460
But then I kind of see and explore how I can sort of weave this into sort of a narrative and a story and then kind of put things in as we go along.
00:05:22.220
And I feel like this makes sense to put this aspect here and put that aspect here so it doesn't come in this sort of, you know, very ABC kind of dry way.
00:05:31.460
So that's my approach and see how I can let things unfold in a natural way.
00:05:36.840
And so you said that when you went to do the recording that the process worked well.
00:05:45.380
So I was very impressed by the whole process, the way things were arranged.
00:05:50.920
Obviously, we were put in a very nice hotel and we felt pampered, honestly.
00:06:01.200
And yeah, and then the shooting itself, people around you, they take care of you, bring you food.
00:06:07.400
They, you know, it's just very, you feel pampered.
00:06:10.620
Yeah, well, you know, we, I think we realized the importance of that, really, when I did the first Exodus seminar for the Daily Wire.
00:06:22.220
We brought nine thinkers in and we spent a fair bit of time on the hospitality side.
00:06:31.580
And one of the things I realized, and I knew this in part from working at a university, was that a lot of the professors that we pulled in for that seminar, I wouldn't say they're exactly well treated at their institutions.
00:06:45.180
And that's foolish because I invited the people who I did invite to the Exodus seminar because I thought they were great and I wanted to hear what they had to say.
00:06:57.400
And there's every reason to make that obvious in every, in every detail of treatment.
00:07:07.720
And so, you know, one of the things I offer people who we offer at Peterson Academy, people who come and lecture, because people ask me, well, you know, what lecture do you need?
00:07:19.380
My sense is, is that we, I find people whose views I want to know and share, and then I want them to do what they think would be best because I wouldn't bloody well invite them if I didn't think they knew what they were doing.
00:07:35.960
And then if we find the right people, we can sort out the curriculum rather than sorting out the curriculum and then forcing the participants, the professors into it.
00:07:48.200
So I would talk about my own research, what I, some of the nicest experiments that I, that I love about my work and sort of weave into that, to that neuroscience curriculum in that way.
00:07:57.800
So we talk about, for example, OCD, and then maybe you might mention the orbitofrontal cortex, which is overactive in OCD.
00:08:05.060
What's the function of the orbitofrontal cortex, this, this, this, the structure in the brain or the striatum and the basal ganglia in the context of Parkinson's.
00:08:13.840
But again, taking in some of some work, that's very, you know, some experiments that, that, that are fascinating.
00:08:20.180
So it's, it's kind of taking that natural approach, that sort of my own research or works of colleagues and just weaving it in, in a more, more natural way, I think.
00:08:30.700
Okay. So I'm curious, everybody who studies the brain in some depth has their own approach.
00:08:39.620
It's like exploring a continent, let's say they have their own approach.
00:08:42.700
And so when you're, when you lay out the architecture of the brain, let's say the basic anatomy, tell me how you do that.
00:08:49.880
Like, I would like to hear how you conceptualize the brain.
00:08:58.400
So for me, I think the way that I, my, my view of the brain was inspired by my mentor, V.S. Romachandran.
00:09:09.760
He's extremely stunningly effective, charismatic.
00:09:13.220
And when I went to California in my early days as an undergraduate, I ended up in his course.
00:09:25.240
So I started there, became a research scholar in his lab eventually.
00:09:30.680
And I think, you know, becoming very good friends with him, ending up in his laboratory, going on, you know, long, long walks on the beach all the time and just spending time with him and getting his view on the brain and sort of adapting that as my own view was, was, was something that, you know, influenced me.
00:09:49.920
And, and, and, and his approach, obviously, and the approach that I've adopted is one where you sort of look at the brain holistically.
00:09:55.800
You acknowledge that the brain is hyperplastic.
00:09:58.640
So there's all these modules that are highly dynamic and the brain is extremely malleable.
00:10:04.060
And so we look at the brain as something that extremely, as, as, as a fluid process that, that's malleable and not fixed and set in its own ways.
00:10:15.680
So that's kind of my, my view of the brain overall, that it's, it's, it's, it's a dynamic object.
00:10:23.140
But beyond that, let me also explain some of our experiments.
00:10:26.680
So the way that we approach science and, and, and probing the human brain is, is through experiments where we look at conceptual experiments in neuroscience.
00:10:36.080
So, for example, there's the robber hand illusion, if you probably know that.
00:10:41.280
So the robber hand illusion is this illusion where you have a chap, he puts his hand right here, and then you have his right hand underneath the table, right?
00:10:49.960
And so me, Balan, the experimenter, will stroke and tap the hand of the, the, the, the sub, the experimental subject.
00:10:57.080
I'll go stroke, stroke, tap, tap, tap, tap, tap, tap, stroke, stroke underneath the table.
00:11:01.060
And I'll stroke and tap the table in front of Joe, the, the subject.
00:11:05.360
I'll go tap, tap, stroke, stroke, stroke, stroke, stroke, tap, tap.
00:11:08.440
And about, I mean, two minutes of me doing this, he will feel touch sensations arising from the table.
00:11:14.100
And I don't mean this in kind of an abstract metaphorical sense.
00:11:16.900
I mean, there's literally, he will have touch sensations, somatosensory regions of his brain becoming active from this process.
00:11:23.800
Yeah, well, that's a very strange element of human perception, right?
00:11:27.360
It, it must be strongly associated not only with our ability to map sensation onto our bodies.
00:11:40.060
Like if I pick up a screwdriver, it takes me virtually no time to use the tip of the screwdriver in a manner that very much approximates the tip of my finger.
00:11:49.740
And then when we go in a car, essentially what we're doing, especially once we're expert drivers, is that we expand the dimensions of our kinesthetic perception, our bodily perception, to include the car.
00:12:02.520
So you're feeling with the tires, you're feeling with the brake.
00:12:06.160
And that's, well, part of my understanding of that is that that's very tightly associated with our tool using, tool using proclivity.
00:12:17.540
So in two minutes, I turn this, this, this subject into a table, right?
00:12:21.960
And then more than that, if I, me, you know, Baland, was to take a hammer and go like this on the table, he'll go, you know, he'll feel pain sensations, right?
00:12:31.860
So the pain regions of his brain will light up if I was to look in a scanner and, you know, look at the neurons there.
00:12:38.800
So it shows you that in that way you can take something, you can, first of all, you can create a sensation of this table belonging to you.
00:12:51.880
But then beyond that, you afflict pain now to the person, to the table, in fact, and then you will feel-
00:12:57.820
Yeah, well, that would also be part of social perception, I presume.
00:13:01.700
I read a paper not long ago, if I remember the details carefully, they were looking at the difference, either, I think it was, I think the dimension was agreeableness.
00:13:14.280
But it might have been psychopathy, which would be the opposite of agreeableness, let's say, that more agreeable people,
00:13:21.880
so less psychopathic people feel, have more pain activation to the perception of other people's pain.
00:13:30.020
So you could imagine that part of the utility in being able to morph your pain sensitivity, even to represent something objective like a table.
00:13:40.100
That's also a variant of my ability to map my own body, let's say, onto your body, so that the empathy that I feel for you isn't conceptual.
00:13:48.960
And I've really been thinking about this in terms of how we understand each other, because it looks to me like what we do to understand each other is, I notice what your aim is, partly by watching your eyes.
00:14:05.900
Once I infer your aim, I can inhabit your perceptual space, because if I know your aim, I know the objects that surround you, but I also know how your emotions are configured.
00:14:15.840
Because they're configured in relationship to the aim.
00:14:18.400
If I can adopt that aim, then I can embody those emotions and perceptions.
00:14:22.740
I can read off that embodiment, and then I, that's, so the understanding is actually my simulating you on my own neural architecture.
00:14:31.160
And then drawing the appropriate inferences from that, and it looks to me like children probably develop that ability.
00:14:41.640
So we actually, we were the first group to show that people with OCD, who has a very fixed sense of self, right, so they wash their hands all the time, washing and scrubbing.
00:14:50.220
When they do this illusion, they have a much more sensitivity to it, to the extent that there's a control condition for this illusion, where you, so the, so the illusion, for the illusion to occur, you have to stroke and tap, tap, tap, stroke, and stroke and tap in a synchronized manner.
00:15:09.480
So you're linking the visual perception to the kinesthetic perception?
00:15:14.200
But in people with OCD, so the control for this, by the way, is if you do it in a random sequence, like tap, tap, stroke, stroke, but everything is just random, right, and you do, again, the touching and stroking is random, then the illusion will not occur, or it will be slightly, so you'll have a slight illusion there, or most of the time, no illusion.
00:15:31.700
That's, so this is the key control for the, for the illusion.
00:15:34.200
Right, so let's just walk through this, so everybody understands.
00:15:38.160
So you have someone with their arm on a table, let's say, their left arm, their right arm is under the table.
00:15:45.260
Now, what you're doing then is you're, you're interacting with their hidden arm physically.
00:15:57.840
So you're syncing their visual perception with their kinesthetic perception.
00:16:01.480
But it's not, but their kinesthetic, or their visual perception isn't focused on their own hand.
00:16:06.040
Now they start to react to the table like it's a hand.
00:16:09.500
Okay, now you're extending this to the OCD situation.
00:16:14.580
And I just want to, just one point here is that I mentioned a table, so the original experiment was done with a rubber hand.
00:16:21.320
But I'm using table because it's more, it illustrates the experiment better, I think, and you can have a table as well.
00:16:28.560
People would start responding to a rubber hand as if it was their own.
00:16:32.060
So instead of a table, you would just have a rubber hand that looks like your own hand, and then you stroke and tap the rubber hand right in front of the person.
00:16:40.580
They start to respond to the rubber hand as if it's theirs.
00:16:48.480
And that can be extended to something as inanimate as a table.
00:17:04.300
So it's stroke, stroke, tap, tap, in the air, and he felt the rubber, like his own hand was floating in the air.
00:17:14.820
Right, you know, that must also be associated with a really profound, with our sense of what constitutes ownership.
00:17:23.980
You know, because the idea that something is yours or that something is mine, there's no reason to assume that that isn't an extension of something like embodiment.
00:17:34.320
It's certainly the case that, you know, if people's cars are attacked, let's say, or kicked, they respond to that very much as if it's a bodily assault.
00:17:44.480
And so it begs the question, how much of our embodied concept of ownership, like that concept on which we platform the philosophical and philosophy and conception of ownership is actually the underlying scaffold for that is our ability to extend our embodiment to even inanimate objects.
00:18:09.520
And part of what you're pointing to with your emphasis on brain plasticity and that is that identity itself, even in terms of perception and pain sensitivity, is fluid and dynamic to a degree that you wouldn't immediately presume.
00:18:26.500
And I do want to differentiate between plasticity and then the dynamic nature of the function of the modules of the brain.
00:18:33.760
So, for example, you have, in terms of the rubber hand illusion, it shows that, for example, we have a structure called the TPJ right here, temporal parietal junction is a fancy name for that.
00:18:43.740
That structure is important for taking all the sensory modalities, touch, hearing, feeling, right?
00:18:49.260
So, smelling and sort of unifying that into a whole.
00:19:00.280
So, it's kind of strategically located between the different sort of occipital somatosensory region and the temporal.
00:19:08.860
So, is it a region that overlaps physiologically between the different sensory integration systems?
00:19:16.860
Is it the same area that's used for silent reading?
00:19:22.700
Well, the reason I'm asking about that from what I remember is that the region that we use for silent reading is the space, is the overlap between the auditory and visual cortex.
00:19:35.120
Because we're basically, when we read silently, we're using our eyes as ears.
00:19:43.020
Okay, so this area is a place between many of this, between a variety of the different sensory integration.
00:19:51.320
And interestingly, actually, it's also involved in the self-other distinction.
00:19:55.420
So, we have a distinction of the land here, Dr. Peterson over there.
00:20:03.040
But that part of the brain, if you zap that, sometimes the self-other distinction can break down.
00:20:07.640
So, you feel like you're merging into another person.
00:20:12.480
It also has connections to the frontal lobes, which is involved in, obviously, in empathy and seeing the perspective of somebody else.
00:20:20.000
So, like a theory of mind, what is Dr. Peterson thinking right now?
00:20:31.060
And this comes to your psychopathy point, actually.
00:20:33.740
Because if you have the temporal parietal junction being involved in body construction, so it's involved in self-constructing a body image, which is expanded in the Robert Hand illusion, but also involved in seeing your perspective as well.
00:20:54.720
Success in business isn't just about offering an amazing product or service, though that's certainly essential.
00:20:59.240
What truly sets thriving companies apart is having powerful, reliable tools working behind the seams to streamline every aspect of the selling process.
00:21:06.700
These are the systems that turn the complex challenge of reaching customers and processing sales into something that feels effortless and natural.
00:21:13.500
That's exactly where Shopify enters the picture, transforming the way businesses operate in the digital age.
00:21:20.640
They're home to the number one checkout on the planet.
00:21:24.220
With ShopPay, they're boosting conversions up to 50%.
00:21:26.780
That means fewer abandoned carts and more sales going to your bottom line.
00:21:29.920
In today's world, your business needs to be everywhere your customers are, whether that's scrolling through social media, shopping online, or walking into a physical store.
00:21:37.400
Shopify powers it all, seamlessly connecting your business across the web, your store, customer feeds, and everywhere in between.
00:21:47.240
Join over 2 million entrepreneurs who have already discovered the power of unified commerce with Shopify's all-in-one platform.
00:21:53.300
Upgrade your business to the same checkout we use with Shopify.
00:21:56.080
Sign up for your $1 per month trial period at shopify.com slash jbp, all lowercase.
00:22:01.060
Head to shopify.com slash jbp to upgrade your selling today.
00:22:06.500
One of the things I thought while you were discussing that is the theory of mind, theories of autism, that autistic people lack theory of mind.
00:22:18.600
But I've never really liked that theory because autism is a really fundamental disorder.
00:22:24.340
Like autistic people generally don't like people and they don't like to be touched.
00:22:34.540
If you don't like to be touched and something's gone wrong at a level that's like under mammal, it's really deep and profound.
00:22:40.800
It's not some, what would you say, alteration in philosophy.
00:22:44.500
But one of the things that is characteristic of autistic people often is that they don't look at people's faces.
00:22:53.440
So then you could imagine it's not exactly a lack of theory of mind.
00:22:58.280
It's that if I don't watch your eyes, I can't infer your aim.
00:23:03.760
If I can't infer your aim, I can't mimic you on my own platform.
00:23:08.940
So I wonder to what degree, I don't know if these experiments have ever been done.
00:23:14.740
I mean, it isn't obvious to me exactly why autistic people don't watch the face.
00:23:23.960
And that is something that seems to upset autistic people.
00:23:27.940
Like furniture moved in a room that they're familiar with.
00:23:31.540
But I wonder what would happen to their ability to experience empathy if they were focally trained to attend to eyes, to learn to perceive the face properly.
00:23:45.120
And so I know Richard Davidson, he's done some studies looking at amygdala activation in autistic children.
00:23:52.320
And they do have an amygdala that's dancing with activity whenever they look at eyes.
00:23:59.860
It probably has to do with the fusiform face area.
00:24:02.340
So there's a region of the brain specialized for recognizing faces.
00:24:15.180
And to be frank, this area of the brain is also involved in dry classification of objects.
00:24:28.100
So it goes like a guitar from a piano or something like that.
00:24:34.080
To have specificity, you have to go higher up in the system.
00:24:42.300
And so obviously in visual processing, you have a hierarchy of where it becomes more complex
00:24:51.840
So then it goes to a point where you start classifying objects in the world.
00:24:58.180
And then after that, you go to vernica, which is more sort of meaning and purpose.
00:25:02.920
And then you go to things like the hippocampus, which is involved in things like memory.
00:25:08.180
So it goes from more simple stages of visual processing to dry classification, faces.
00:25:15.060
Dr. Peterson's from Alex, from Kim, knowing different people.
00:25:27.920
I was really interested in his work for a long while, especially on hemispheric lateralization and neglect.
00:25:37.180
Now, some of the experiments that you described emerged because of Ramachandran's investigations into neglect, right, originally.
00:25:47.140
So neglect, for those of you who are watching and listening, is a very strange phenomenon.
00:25:50.940
So if you have a stroke and you have, correct me if I get any of this wrong because it's been like 20 years since I thought about it, right parietal damage.
00:26:01.480
You'll lose your perception of the left side of your body.
00:26:05.600
But more than that, this is where it gets very weird.
00:26:11.940
And I've tried to imagine it sort of like, you know how everything that's behind you when you're looking forward, it isn't like it's missing.
00:26:21.720
It's just simply not there at a level that's even more profound than missing.
00:26:26.200
And I suspect what happens to people with right parietal damage is that the absence that characterizes your lack of perception of what's behind you extends so that now it's three quarters of the world instead of half.
00:26:40.740
Anyways, weirdly enough, you lose your ability to perceive the left side of everything.
00:26:49.920
But one of the consequences of that is that people with profound neglect will wake up and they'll become aware of their left arm or their left leg after they've had a stroke.
00:27:00.920
And they'll try to throw it out of bed because they think it's someone else's.
00:27:04.860
And if you get them to draw a clock, for example, they'll draw half a clock and cram all the numbers into the right-hand side.
00:27:10.540
And if you give them a plate, they'll eat half the food.
00:27:13.500
I still can't figure this out because how do you think that works?
00:27:17.480
It's like if I pick up my phone, now I look at the phone.
00:27:27.400
Now, if I'm looking at the room now, do I miss the left side of the room?
00:27:33.980
And in the room, do I miss the left side of all the objects?
00:27:37.680
Like I just don't understand how the hell that works perceptually.
00:27:45.020
They can draw a flower the whole day, but they only draw half of the flower.
00:27:50.320
And you keep telling them and they say, well, I did my best.
00:27:53.360
But they can't attend to that part of the brain.
00:27:56.240
Mind you, the parietal lobes is involved in spatial orientation, knowing not only the body
00:28:00.900
where it is in space, but also the spatial layout of the room, right?
00:28:09.240
And in order to understand how they are experiencing this at a subjective level is really critical.
00:28:18.440
Rabbi Chandran also did experiments with irrigation, didn't he?
00:28:23.900
But before we go there, so the parietal lobes and the superior parietal lobule, another fancy
00:28:30.520
So just above the temporal parietal junction is specifically involved in creating a body
00:28:37.160
So the TPJ we talked about, taking information from various sensory modalities and then whispering
00:28:43.280
information to the superior parietal lobule, this area just above it, its neighbor, right?
00:28:48.740
It's involved in creating a subjective sense of a self, the feeling that I occupy this body
00:28:55.360
So when people have a stroke to that part of the brain, as you mentioned, they will be
00:29:00.500
will sometimes throw their hands out and say, this arm doesn't belong to me, it belongs
00:29:05.040
to you, or it belongs to my dad, or it belongs to, you know, this person or that person.
00:29:12.740
You can play chess with them, you can have conversation, nothing, nothing is wrong.
00:29:17.040
Otherwise, they're not delusional, they're not psychotic or anything like that.
00:29:20.780
But after they have the stroke affect this region of the brain, they will just say that
00:29:26.720
Or sometimes they might even say, you might ask them, you say, well, they might deny the
00:29:44.280
And they will take the lifeless arm and lift it like this and say, I'm touching it, doctor.
00:29:51.300
Yeah, well, it's almost as if, I wondered too, is it a lack of capacity to update as
00:29:59.040
Like, it seems to me that what must be happening is they're using a pre-stroke representation
00:30:06.360
And the tissue that's been destroyed normally would update that.
00:30:10.540
Because I remember too, with Ramachandran's experiments, when he irrigated, this is very
00:30:15.160
weird to everyone, Ramachandran would irrigate the ears, so pour water and cold water in the
00:30:22.220
ears of left ear, if I remember correctly, left ear, of the people who had neglect, and
00:30:28.880
Now, that disturbed their vestibular system, which is involved with bodily orientation.
00:30:33.540
And it would shock them into the realization that they had a paralyzed left side.
00:30:39.740
And they would break down emotionally, catastrophically, with the realization that they'd been so badly
00:30:47.620
But then the effects of the irrigation would wear off, and they'd snap back into this.
00:30:53.520
And that's why I think it's not exactly a delusional state.
00:30:56.180
I think they're stuck with the body representation that existed before the stroke.
00:31:01.340
And what's been eradicated, the systems that could update that, the right hemisphere systems,
00:31:08.920
So there's no way of fixing, there's no straightforward way of fixing it.
00:31:13.840
So one way to approach this, or sort of think about this, is that, you know, the left side
00:31:18.540
of the brain, the left hemisphere and the right hemisphere have different functions.
00:31:21.400
I just want to make it clear to the listeners, I know you know this, right?
00:31:26.440
So intriguingly, it's only in the right side, if you have the stroke in the left, they will
00:31:33.760
So this tells you there's something going on about self in the rights that's obviously different.
00:31:41.060
Interestingly, if you have a stroke to the prefrontal on the left side, you will develop
00:31:47.680
So you might have a conversation and start crying in the middle of the conversation without
00:31:54.360
If you get a stroke in the right prefrontal, you will have become delusionally optimistic.
00:32:01.040
So you'll go out and buy a Rolex if you can't, you know, and get, you know, become manic
00:32:05.440
So it shows us that the left hemisphere is involved in positive emotion and the right is
00:32:12.100
And in fact, today, when you use things like TMS, transcranial magnetic stimulation, you might
00:32:17.120
zap the left hemisphere in people with depression and make it more active and you end up with
00:32:23.680
So the brain has these functions that are very lateralized and highly unique to each side
00:32:30.800
Well, so L. Conan Goldberg, I really liked his model of brain lateralization.
00:32:39.000
And I know Ramachandran developed a theory that was either parallel or influenced by Goldberg.
00:32:45.940
But what that would imply, if the right hemisphere is associated with novelty recognition, so it
00:32:55.440
It makes sense that it would signal negative emotion because the first thing that should
00:32:59.340
happen when something you don't expect occurs, because that means the routine you're running
00:33:07.320
Because what I'm trying to do is whenever I run a perceptual routine, I have a goal in
00:33:12.580
mind, and I'm presuming that my perception is adequate to the task.
00:33:17.880
If something anomalous occurs, like if I tell you a joke and you don't find it funny, or maybe
00:33:22.600
I tell you a joke and you get offended by it, then obviously the way I've mapped you is wrong.
00:33:32.520
That's going to be signaled by the right hemisphere, anomaly, negative emotion.
00:33:40.260
Now, the problem with depressed people is they attend to it catastrophically.
00:33:45.820
So like, say if I'm depressed and I make a mistake like that with you, we're sitting
00:33:51.080
here talking, I make a little joke, and you either don't find it funny or you act offended,
00:33:59.400
A depressed person would think, well, that didn't go over very well.
00:34:08.520
Obviously, I don't understand people very, I don't understand this person very well.
00:34:13.700
Oh, that's because I really don't understand anyone very well.
00:34:17.860
I didn't understand people very well in the past, and I don't understand them well, and
00:34:22.560
I'm very unlikely to learn how to do that in the future.
00:34:26.520
People who are unable to learn like that socially, they're not very useful people.
00:34:33.220
Some people are so useless that it would be better if they weren't around at all.
00:34:39.240
And so I'm wondering if the, so you can imagine the right hemisphere when it's analyzing
00:34:43.840
something novel, opens up that entire space of potential consideration.
00:34:49.540
My suspicions are that the left prefrontal cortex probably puts a box around that continually.
00:34:56.860
You know, because the right level of analysis, if I make a joke with you and it falls flat,
00:35:03.740
I should note that, but I shouldn't leap to catastrophic conclusions to begin with.
00:35:09.820
I should just note it, like it should now become a, what would you say, an object of potential
00:35:17.120
And I've noticed in my clinical practice that people who are well regulated emotionally
00:35:21.940
won't undergo a detailed investigation into an anomaly until it repeats, let's say, three
00:35:30.580
Whereas depressed people, they'll leap to the worst possible conclusion almost immediately.
00:35:34.700
And that does look like something like, like that left hemisphere, left prefrontal hemispheric
00:35:41.500
So you can imagine that the right hemisphere notes the novelty, elicits negative emotion,
00:35:48.200
then opens up the search space, which could be indefinite.
00:35:51.480
Like the reason your joke didn't go over might be because you are the kind of unpopular loser
00:36:00.160
But that shouldn't be your first conclusion, right?
00:36:03.480
So you lose the left hemisphere system and that whole cataclysmic reaction is dysregulated.
00:36:11.520
So the right hemisphere is more emotional, big picture oriented, as you say.
00:36:15.860
And it could be that it goes into this infinite loop of possibilities in this big space land
00:36:25.120
Okay, so I would say I'd like to know your thoughts about, so I'm very interested in archetypal
00:36:36.760
And Carl Jung had a very specific hypothesis about dreams, which I really like.
00:36:42.920
And this is one of the things I wanted to talk to you about today because you're interested
00:36:46.160
in dreams and you've talked about them as simulations.
00:36:49.100
So his idea about dreams, which is a brilliant idea, is that it wasn't hemispherically localized
00:36:55.580
for him because he didn't have the neuroanatomical knowledge.
00:37:05.380
You could say that that exploration that we just described, which is, you know, is there
00:37:18.020
And you could think of the fantasies as attempts to remap the anomalous situation.
00:37:24.700
And then, like, a shallow remapping would require just a tiny alteration of fantasy,
00:37:32.280
but a large remapping would mean a whole reconfiguration of character.
00:37:36.500
Jung's idea was that the dream was a place of exploration for the remapping of anomaly
00:37:43.020
and that it could be undertaken safely because you could explore different perceptual configurations
00:37:51.380
in the safety of dreams without exposing yourself to any danger.
00:37:54.700
So you could imagine that, so the right hemisphere signifies, detects anomaly and it begins this
00:38:04.760
exploration process, but it's using the landscape of fantasy, which would be simulation, to start
00:38:15.620
And you could imagine, too, that one of the ways that that might be triangulated would be, imagine
00:38:23.620
that your right hemisphere has aggregated a couple of different anomalies.
00:38:28.760
Not enough to be cataclysmic about them, but to have them sort of there as mysteries.
00:38:34.460
Okay, now you search through the fantasy space, and one of the new fantasies explains, like,
00:38:43.240
My guess is that's something like fantasy-related insight.
00:38:47.500
Because you'd see that in therapy, you know, where someone will lay out a couple of different
00:38:55.800
And then contemplate the commonalities, and sometimes they'll stumble across something
00:39:02.420
that, oh, I see, I'm looking at this whole thing wrong.
00:39:06.240
It's a restructuring of the theory, and then those three anomalies are all accounted for.
00:39:10.840
And that's going to give you a sense of conviction, right?
00:39:13.020
Because now you have a theory that accounts for the new information, and the dream is part
00:39:18.300
It's the birthplace of that, the birthplace of that re-novelization of conception.
00:39:25.820
So I think definitely, in terms of using the right hemisphere in dreams, there's got to
00:39:32.100
So I'm not sure how much this has been explored in terms of the right hemisphere only.
00:39:35.940
But that's definitely because dreams is so much about emotional updating and emotional creating
00:39:42.020
a sense of, making sense of an emotional landscape, right?
00:39:47.180
So in dreams, for example, it's heavily populated by people, right?
00:39:53.100
So each night, you cycle through different stages of sleep, stage one, two, three, and
00:39:57.980
then you have deep sleep, and then you have REM sleep, rapid eye movement sleep, where your
00:40:04.380
During this stage of sleep, you're paralyzed from head to toe, so you have structures in the
00:40:08.040
brain called the pons and the medulla in the lower part of the brain that paralyzes your
00:40:12.100
entire body so you won't act out your dreams and hurt yourself, right?
00:40:14.920
So this is, so I want to lay the foundation before I get to your point, right?
00:40:18.400
Yeah, and that sets up the stage for exploration without risk.
00:40:21.040
Right, so now you can engage in this laboratory of testing, a testing space without any fatal
00:40:29.180
You can do whatever you want and you don't hurt your sleeping partner or yourself.
00:40:34.500
You're in a physiological straitjacket, so to speak, right?
00:40:37.180
Yeah, and you can explore deeply enough so that you can actually reshape not just your
00:40:41.660
conceptions, but your perceptions, and that idea accounts for some of the bizarreness of
00:40:47.880
Like, if you're exploring at the level of perception, it's going to seem bizarre, obviously.
00:40:56.920
So first of all, you're paralyzed in REM sleep, right?
00:41:01.440
Your eyelids can move because of a different circuit, by the way.
00:41:04.640
So this is a different circuit for the eyelids.
00:41:06.200
It's now, interestingly, the emotional part of the brain, the limbic centers tucked behind
00:41:13.600
Your prefrontal lobes, and the CEO of the brain, becomes less active for some reason.
00:41:26.660
And so everything in the world becomes less focused, right?
00:41:34.000
So you don't think in a logical, straightforward, ABC-like manner, right?
00:41:38.340
So if I wake somebody up from REM sleep and say, well, and tell him the word sun, he will
00:41:47.560
So he doesn't think in a logical, serial manner.
00:41:52.080
In fact, he will be more likely to say sun and chair versus when somebody's awake and
00:41:56.340
I ask him, well, what do you think of now when I say sun?
00:42:00.060
So they are more likely to relate unrelated words.
00:42:05.360
Which is what you'd expect if it was an exploratory process.
00:42:12.220
The emotional part of the brain dial up, right?
00:42:17.220
Now, this is a perfect cocktail for strange things.
00:42:20.400
Not only that, but also the chemicals in your brain that have to do with logical and linear
00:42:25.920
thinking noradrenaline, you have adrenaline in your body when you're anxious or you're
00:42:33.040
You have noradrenaline in the brain, but also in the body.
00:42:35.560
But noradrenaline is a cousin chemical of adrenaline.
00:42:42.920
Are you tired of being held back by one-size-fits-all healthcare?
00:42:46.560
Of having your concerns dismissed or being denied that comprehensive lab work, you need to
00:42:52.160
I want to tell you about Merrick Health, the premier health optimization platform that's
00:42:56.040
revolutionizing how we approach wellness and longevity.
00:42:58.980
What sets Merrick apart isn't just their cutting-edge diagnostic labs or concierge health
00:43:02.940
coaching, it's their commitment to treating you as an individual.
00:43:05.960
Their expert clinical team stays at the forefront of medical research, creates personalized,
00:43:12.780
Unlike other services that rely on cookie-cutter solutions, Merrick Health goes the extra mile.
00:43:17.060
They consider your unique lifestyle, blood work, and goals to craft recommendations that actually
00:43:22.160
Whether that's through lifestyle modifications, supplementation, or prescription treatments.
00:43:26.620
And with a remarkable 4.9 out of 5 rating on Trustpilot, you know you're in great hands.
00:43:31.680
The best part is you can get 10% off your order today.
00:43:34.180
Just head to merrickhealth.com and use code PETERSON at checkout.
00:43:37.220
That's merrickhealth.com, code PETERSON for 10% off.
00:43:40.240
Stop guessing and start optimizing your health today with Merrick Health.
00:43:43.420
Because your best life starts with your best health.
00:43:45.940
It turns out there's a structure in the brain, in the brainstem, called the locus coeruleus.
00:44:01.680
They will stop, you know, secreting noradrenaline during REM sleep.
00:44:07.420
So that means your way of thinking about the world is unfocused.
00:44:20.860
So this is a perfect cocktail now for craziness.
00:44:24.320
The vestibular part of your brain become hyperactive.
00:44:27.120
You know, the central motor region of the brain that has to do with automatic sort of behavior
00:44:35.580
So that's why dreams, you can sometimes feel like you're running, but you can't control your
00:44:39.720
You feel like some monster is chasing you, but you can't move or you feel like you're
00:44:48.160
And that's because the parts of the brain that has to do with automatic movements, sporadically,
00:45:00.240
And it seems like then it's just perfect for what you're saying then that the right hemisphere
00:45:05.220
type of thinking of exploration and emotion is just, it's hyperactive.
00:45:11.800
Right, so it's a safe place for simulation, for exploratory simulation that can be so extreme
00:45:21.780
Why not explore the outer limits under safe conditions?
00:45:24.780
Well, I've also, I remember that if you wake people up from REM sleep, the most common
00:45:32.260
And that makes sense too, if you think about this as part of the, what would you say, the
00:45:39.100
reconfiguration in the face of novelty process.
00:45:43.980
Because the most appropriate first response to something anomalous is anxiety.
00:45:51.580
It's to, because technically what anxiety does is stop the operation of, it stops current
00:45:59.280
So that's like a prey, it's like a prey animal response.
00:46:05.000
Because what you're doing has either not worked or exposed you to danger.
00:46:09.080
So now you're not where you thought you were and what you're doing isn't doing what
00:46:22.920
So like if you throw a rat into a new cage, the first thing it does is freeze.
00:46:29.360
Then what it starts to do is to look around, I guess to begin with, it doesn't even want
00:46:34.740
Because it doesn't want to attack, attract the attention of a predator.
00:46:40.380
And then if nothing additionally terrible happens, it starts to thaw and starts to look and starts
00:46:49.040
And then it'll start to explore and remap the territory.
00:46:52.840
But that seems to be what's happening at the dream level conceptually.
00:46:57.380
So you imagine that you're encountering a landscape of anomaly or novelty that's signified by the
00:47:03.840
The emotion that's elicited is anxiety with a subtext of curiosity.
00:47:09.360
Because both of those two things would be at play.
00:47:11.640
And then the fantasy landscape can be elaborated so that even perception itself can be reconfigured
00:47:20.140
If you're betrayed by someone, you could say something like, I can't even look at you the
00:47:26.400
And that means that the betrayal has forced a reconception so profound that perception
00:47:38.840
You could find someone attractive or unattractive on first meeting.
00:47:42.720
And then as you get to know them, maybe you thought they were attractive to begin with
00:47:49.060
Or you felt that they weren't that attractive to begin with, but as you get to know their
00:47:53.340
character, let's say, then the perception itself shifts.
00:47:57.360
It's not merely the theory of mind or the conception you have of them, right?
00:48:04.460
Maybe it's because in part you actually see, I don't know, if you reconfigure the, what
00:48:11.460
would be the patterns of interaction in their face.
00:48:14.700
You know, like someone graceful, for example, you're obviously perceiving something like
00:48:19.500
And there's something charming and beautiful about that.
00:48:21.940
My guess is if you see a person of high character across time, you can see their integration.
00:48:27.640
And that that would make them, that would allow you to perceive their attractiveness in a
00:48:32.040
way that you might not have been able to do superficially.
00:48:36.880
Well, to go on back to the whole dream thing, right?
00:48:39.720
So one of the things is that it's populated by a lot of people and that's, again, right
00:48:44.620
hemisphere is actually involved in decoding social, like facial expressions, for example.
00:48:50.400
So that's one thing that obviously autistic people have problems with.
00:48:54.240
But for some reason, there's a lot of faith, there's a lot of people, a lot of interactions
00:49:00.280
And usually, actually, these interactions are negative.
00:49:02.860
And this shows us that for some reason, it's advantageous to treat me of negative things
00:49:09.240
because you're more likely to train your circuits in your brain to be able to, so to put it
00:49:16.980
shortly, like if I have an encounter with a serial killer in my dream and I sort of overcome
00:49:26.080
I can navigate that situation in an appropriate manner.
00:49:31.560
So it shows you that dreams has a lot to do with survival and training the circuits in
00:49:36.960
the brain, making them solidify the ones that can help me survive more.
00:49:42.500
Well, and it would make sense, too, that what you should...
00:49:45.920
Look, the more sophisticated you are in your social perception, the less likely even encounters
00:50:02.480
So I had this landlord when I lived in Montreal, and he was an ex-president of Hell's Angels
00:50:10.120
And he'd been in prison, and he was a rough guy.
00:50:14.260
And we couldn't communicate that easily because he spoke joual French, which is very hard
00:50:23.880
And there was quite a big class difference between us.
00:50:27.400
And I was from Western Canada, and she was from Eastern Canada.
00:50:34.720
And to the degree we could communicate, we did.
00:50:43.660
And we kind of got to know each other insofar as we could.
00:50:50.500
The problem was that now and then he would go on a bender, and he could drink, well,
00:50:56.580
like an unlimited amount of alcohol over a three-day period, like 90 beer, you know?
00:51:01.060
Like, and he would drink himself to a point where it was not even obvious how conscious
00:51:13.680
And then he developed a habit of coming to my door at like three in the morning to sell
00:51:17.980
me like a toaster or a microwave because he needed money because he wanted to keep drinking.
00:51:33.160
He put me on the back of his motorcycle, which is a 750 Honda, if I remember correctly.
00:51:37.560
He put his wife's helmet on my head, which is his little tiny helmet, and away we went.
00:51:41.220
He said, if the cops come after me, I'm not stopping, just so you know.
00:51:45.460
It's like, well, that was the beginning of a very interesting evening.
00:51:47.980
And he got into all sorts of fights at the bars because people would come up and like
00:52:01.040
Well, I talked to my wife about this because she didn't like the fact that I was giving
00:52:06.880
Paulo money for his like toasters and so forth because she knew that he was trying to quit
00:52:12.400
So, and then it also scared her that he would come over like at three in the morning.
00:52:16.080
So one day he came over at three in the morning and he was standing there sort of swaying
00:52:23.740
And I had to tell him that I wasn't going to give him any more money starting then.
00:52:30.340
And so I said, look, we know you're trying to quit drinking.
00:52:35.900
And when you come over and I buy your toaster, then you go and drink.
00:52:40.980
And I can't do that anymore because it's not good.
00:52:43.680
And he looked at me for like 10 seconds and I know why he was looking at me.
00:52:47.580
He was looking at me to see if I was playing moral superiority games.
00:52:54.480
In which case the interaction wouldn't have gone very well.
00:52:58.480
And so the reason I'm bringing that up is because as your social perception becomes
00:53:04.420
more sophisticated, the probability that you can navigate well in a complex and potentially
00:53:15.160
Now, that should mean that you should concentrate on elements of social behavior that didn't
00:53:23.460
And that would take you into a landscape of, well, hard to tell, but possibly into a landscape
00:53:29.060
That would explain, for example, why people go and watch horror movies and movies about
00:53:39.460
And so the fact is that, you know, when you dream that your brain takes you on this exploration,
00:53:45.020
And it looks at various social scenarios, for example, that evoke emotions in you.
00:53:50.060
So it takes, you know, Dr. Peterson and put him in a room with Kim and Joe and see how
00:53:56.920
If the reaction is not an emotionally evocative one, it will literally take you and show you
00:54:03.160
another scenario until it hits on a scene that evokes your emotion that gets you riled
00:54:11.100
It has, yeah, it has to have that element and then it will go down that path more and
00:54:23.980
Now, but that, but is it also searching for emotional reactions that are primarily negative
00:54:34.260
So it does, there's a, there's a huge dopaminergic aspects to dreams.
00:54:38.620
So it's been shown that if you have a lesion to a part of the brain, the inferior parietal
00:54:42.500
lobule, again, it's a region just below the superior parietal lobule is involved in
00:54:46.480
creating images, but also it's, so if you have a stroke there, for example, you won't
00:54:50.420
dream or the, the, the, the mesolimbic dopamine centers is a fancy name for the part
00:54:54.940
of the brain where you have dopamine going to the prefrontal cortex.
00:54:58.180
If you have a lesion there, you won't dream as well.
00:55:00.620
So bliss and dopamine, as well as images is involved deeply in, in, in.
00:55:12.680
We are generally as human beings tilted somewhat toward the negative.
00:55:17.580
So your brain is playing with various scenarios.
00:55:21.740
And if one evokes an emotional response, so you see, the thing is, if it evokes a negative
00:55:28.840
emotional response, that would indicate that your adaptation is weak at that point, right?
00:55:33.040
Because you're much more likely to be anxious about a situation that you haven't mastered.
00:55:39.380
So you could imagine that the evocation of negative emotion is indication of weakness
00:55:49.140
So now the dream is playing with various scenarios around that.
00:55:56.700
So one thing that I want to make clear as well, it's that, you know, obviously, as you
00:56:00.400
know, there's a corpus callosum that, that, that there's a bridge between the two hemispheres,
00:56:04.300
allowing the two hemisphere to, to, to communicate.
00:56:06.780
So you have the right hemisphere and the left hemisphere.
00:56:09.160
So these hemisphere, if, if you literally cut it, there won't, you'll have two consciousness
00:56:15.140
So it seems like dreams is also right hemispheric dominance for another reason, because the
00:56:21.280
things you will see in your dreams are, are like poetry, right?
00:56:25.920
It's visual metaphors that you can't explain in language.
00:56:29.280
So I can, it's like going through an art museum and looking at things, but in a very poetically,
00:56:40.060
And obviously the left hemisphere, the regions of the Wernicke and stuff like that is involved
00:56:46.460
But it seems like, like the poetic aspect of dreams is very much a right hemispheric,
00:56:54.440
That, that should be associated with both the novelty routinization dichotomy.
00:57:00.980
So the question is, well, how do you approach something that's novel?
00:57:04.700
Well, if it's novel, if it produces anomaly, if you don't understand it, you haven't encapsulated
00:57:10.700
It's not routinized enough so that you have a propositional description of it.
00:57:16.200
So you can imagine that during the day when you're conscious, you're running well routinized,
00:57:26.600
But some of them don't work out as well as others.
00:57:31.460
So now imagine the right hemisphere is sitting in the background, mapping the failures.
00:57:41.620
You shut down the propositional side and you open up the metaphoric side and it's starting
00:57:49.440
I think it's exploring unexplored territory fundamentally.
00:57:54.540
But it's also like the land, it's the same as the landscape of insufficient adaptation.
00:58:00.440
And so, and that should be associated with negative emotion.
00:58:06.300
And now the question would be, what would be the utility of those metaphoric fantasies?
00:58:14.840
So imagine that around any perception, there's a cloud of connotations, right?
00:58:27.360
So like a first order connotation for you would, if I saw you would obviously be male.
00:58:33.700
So, and then imagine that outside of that, there's second order connotations and third
00:58:39.360
order and finally things that are so distinct that they don't seem to bear any conceptual
00:58:44.700
Well, as you open up the metaphoric landscape, that connotation width should expand.
00:58:51.480
And then you could imagine that what you're trying to do is to explore a new network of
00:58:58.500
connotations that would map the territory more effectively, right?
00:59:04.340
I think, and I think what is also interesting about dreams and that whole thing is that
00:59:08.660
it seems to tap into a circuitry that's more mystical than the circuitry that we normally
00:59:15.280
So by mystical, I mean, it seems like some of the receptors involved in mystical experiences
00:59:22.060
when you take psilocybin and things like mescaline and DMT and stuff like that, the serotonin
00:59:29.420
So one theory actually talks about how, so obviously serotonin is another neurochemical in the brain
00:59:36.120
that the part of the brain that produces that, the dorsal raffin nucleus, also shuts down
00:59:42.180
So you don't have serotonin in your dreams either in REM sleep.
00:59:45.280
Um, and, and so you end up in this space without noradrenaline and without serotonin, but it
00:59:50.540
seems like for some reason that the serotonin 2A receptors become, become dialed up.
00:59:56.320
So that part of the serotonin 2A receptors become tickled for some reasons.
01:00:01.060
And that is also happening in a psilocybin experience.
01:00:05.980
So that accounts in part for the overlap between the mystical experience and the dream experience.
01:00:10.160
The mystical experiences and the dream experiences.
01:00:12.140
So there's something there that is, that's hyper-cosmic in dreams that you can't, it's
01:00:17.980
You can't describe it in language and even, and it's highly personal and it has salience
01:00:23.340
So one thing that I noticed about dream is a lot of people come and talk to me about
01:00:27.840
They'll go, Baland, you know, I had this dream.
01:00:29.660
I saw this and that, and they're, they're, they're very emotional about it.
01:00:35.280
But, you know, I kind of go, oh, that's interesting, but it's not really that interesting, but
01:00:43.940
It's, it's, it has personal salience, um, kind of the, the type of personal and salience
01:00:48.760
salience you can get from a psilocybin experience.
01:00:52.260
Can you believe that Good Friday and Easter are just around the corner?
01:00:55.380
These are the most important holidays in Christianity and Lent is our time to prepare our hearts.
01:01:00.080
Lent is traditionally a time where Christians grow closer to God through prayer, fasting
01:01:06.040
Hallow has created an amazing way to do just that with their Lent prayer 40 challenge called
01:01:14.200
This challenge helps us remember that Jesus is the way to heaven, showing us that true
01:01:18.280
peace, love, and joy require genuine sacrifice.
01:01:22.380
Mark Wahlberg and Chris Pratt are leading some incredible challenges about sacrifice and surrender.
01:01:26.820
Jonathan Rumi and sister Miriam James will guide you through these powerful stories and
01:01:32.380
And Father Mike Schmitz, his Sunday sermons are absolutely spectacular.
01:01:36.380
This is already shaping up to be Hallow's biggest Lent ever with thousands of people from all
01:01:43.460
You can get three months completely free when you sign up at hallow.com slash Jordan.
01:01:49.800
Once you join, you'll discover a huge catalog of guided prayers, meditations, music, and so much
01:01:54.620
more, all designed to help you grow closer to God and find the peace that we're all looking
01:01:59.400
So download the Hallow app today and jump into the Lent Prayer 40 Challenge.
01:02:10.460
Okay, so you said something paradoxical about that, right?
01:02:13.240
That it's a weird combination of intensely personal and cosmic, which means intensely universal.
01:02:21.160
That's a strange conjunction because those two things are actually quite far apart.
01:02:26.040
The more personal something is, in some ways, the less cosmic, the less universal it is.
01:02:33.760
So I wonder, see, an archetype in some ways bridges the gap between the personal and the
01:02:43.300
So here's an example of something you can do with a dream that's very effective.
01:02:48.720
So imagine that you have a client who's chased by something awful in a dream, and that repeats.
01:03:00.380
And then you ask them what they do, and they say, well, I run away or I try to hide.
01:03:05.680
So then you say to them, well, here's what we're going to do instead.
01:03:09.080
I want you to sit there, close your eyes, then make them relax so that they get into a state
01:03:17.520
Okay, now, close your eyes, bring the dream to mind.
01:03:23.000
So they'll replay the dream and say, now, okay, now, at the end, instead of running,
01:03:28.040
tell me what happens if you turn around and ask the criminal or the sadist or the monster
01:03:37.960
what it's up to, or what happens if you approach it instead of running.
01:03:44.960
Well, and then the dream will continue in their imagination most of the time.
01:03:49.660
And generally, what occurs in a consequence of that is that dream goes away.
01:03:55.060
Now, I think the reason for that, and I think this is akin to this bridging of the gap between
01:03:59.320
the personal and the cosmic is because their personal response to being chased is to run
01:04:10.180
Now, if you transform that into voluntary explorers,
01:04:17.180
then what you're doing is you're inculcating an element of the hero myth into the dream
01:04:26.440
It's like, no, the right strategy when you're threatened isn't to run.
01:04:30.840
The right strategy is to turn voluntarily and to commence the process of exploration.
01:04:39.520
So the monster in your dream represents your hyperactive amygdala and the limbic centers
01:04:45.300
of the brain being hyperactive, up to 30% more active in the dream landscape, right?
01:04:52.920
And by the way, the hippocampus also turns out the memory part of the brain is also hyperactive
01:04:59.240
So you have the memory spilling in into this narrative about a monster chasing you, right?
01:05:04.500
So you give it identity, you give it name, you give it, you know, all these negative
01:05:12.440
You contextualize it based on the hyperactive hippocampus.
01:05:15.940
Then it's chasing you and you can't run away because we said the motor, central motor generator
01:05:20.640
of your brainstem is making it very hard for you to move.
01:05:24.040
So you don't have the, so movement normally occurs in the motor cortex of your brain that
01:05:29.860
But because that part of the brain can compete with the central motor, automatic part of the
01:05:35.140
brain firing and making your behaviors all, you know, sporadic and automatic.
01:05:42.760
You said, if you turn around and approach the monster, the monster will be, it will become
01:05:49.420
And that's interesting because we know in the real world, if you walk simply by walking,
01:05:54.980
you will turn down the activity of the amygdala because you're telling your, you're telling
01:05:58.840
your brain or your, yourself that you are approaching, you're engaging in approach behavior instead
01:06:05.440
Which, which puts you, see, that shift your identity in relationship to the thing that's
01:06:10.860
You're changing and saying, now I'm no longer the prey here.
01:06:14.760
I am the one that is being, that, that is doing the haunting, you know?
01:06:19.020
And so in that sense, it would make sense that the monster would vanish.
01:06:25.020
So that's very interesting, but I also want to touch on that whole, on monsters, since
01:06:30.620
So there's a condition called sleep paralysis, and I talk about it in my, my Peterson Academy
01:06:53.320
Most of my experience was being unable to move and no, I knew what sleep paralysis was.
01:07:04.500
So even when it happened to be in my dreams, usually my experience was that I, something
01:07:11.300
was happening to me and I was frozen and unable to speak.
01:07:20.320
I mean, literally in my life, I, I would yell and she'd have to come and shake me and then
01:07:25.120
But no, I didn't have the monster element to it.
01:07:32.380
I think the reason that didn't happen is because I knew what was happening.
01:07:35.820
So the, the monster too, just out of curiosity.
01:07:39.280
So you could imagine that with this interplay between the hippocampus and the amygdala, if
01:07:46.900
So now there's lots of emotions being triggered.
01:07:49.140
Now the memory systems are interacting with those emotions.
01:07:55.900
Here's the thing that would be most likely to manifest that.
01:07:58.920
So if it's a panoply of emotions, it would be an amalgam of emotion evoking stimuli.
01:08:05.260
And there's no difference between an amalgam of emotion, eliciting stimuli and a monster.
01:08:14.760
So a monster is your worst nightmare come to life.
01:08:18.500
It's whatever you dread, whatever is lurking in your unconscious mind, that's coming to
01:08:24.120
And so during sleep paralysis, interestingly, you didn't have any of the monsters, but it
01:08:29.020
turns out about 40% of people will see monsters.
01:08:34.800
So you have this REM paralysis, obviously you're paralyzed from head to toe during REM
01:08:40.240
Occasionally for some people, they might start feeling like they can, you know, they can see
01:08:46.260
So they might open their eyes and then they realize, my God, I'm paralyzed.
01:08:53.900
And then they look around and I had one of these experiences.
01:08:59.340
So I was sleeping in my room as a teenager in Copenhagen.
01:09:02.320
And I grew up in a ghetto-like neighborhood in Copenhagen.
01:09:07.780
And then I woke up paralyzed, unable to move or speak.
01:09:11.100
And then I had this creepy feeling of a monster from the corner of my room approaching me.
01:09:15.820
And it came closer and closer until it was on my chest, strangling me.
01:09:23.700
And mind you, at this point, I was like, is this real?
01:09:27.040
It was as crisp as this conversation you and I are having right now.
01:09:31.740
So at this point, I just saw my legs flying up and down.
01:09:45.740
But I've had sleep paralysis since then on many occasions.
01:09:51.000
So I've seen, like one was Colonel Gaddafi, you know him, eccentric figure.
01:09:54.880
He was hovering over me when I was living in Egypt for a period of time in my early undergraduate years.
01:10:02.040
And I saw during the Libyan Revolution, of all people, Gaddafi was in my bedroom, hovering over me, you know, attacking me.
01:10:12.980
So he's the monster of the oppressive patriarchy coming to visit.
01:10:18.180
My wife had a dream like that about Richard Nixon dressed in a general's outfit.
01:10:26.580
Like, you could think about him as the monstrous form of the patriarchy.
01:10:38.380
Because the monstrous element of the patriarchy is the negative side of the social order.
01:10:51.340
And in hero mythology, of course, one of the categories of monster that the hero fights
01:11:01.400
That's more like a representation of the terrible aspect of nature.
01:11:12.280
Around that time, there was a spring, the Arab Spring and the Libyan Revolution.
01:11:16.320
And all that was going on and going on and I was watching CNN all the time, you know?
01:11:24.460
I was sort of watching the news and, you know, I was influenced in some way.
01:11:30.020
So our research now in about seven countries has shown that, you know, whenever you have
01:11:35.300
a cultural narrative for it, like stories of witches, of space aliens or whatever, you
01:11:42.640
will have those lurking into your unconscious and you will see that manifest, right?
01:11:50.740
Those are your Carl Jung archetype, you know, figures appearing.
01:11:57.080
So, for example, in Egypt, we showed that the evil genius of Aladdin, you know, Aladdin
01:12:01.460
the cartoon, you will have that appear in front of you.
01:12:04.080
So, you know, they will have bloody fangs and everything will be creepy, very, you know,
01:12:10.180
In Italy, for example, in the Pandafika region of Italy, you will see these giant cats or witches.
01:12:25.380
Okay, so what that would imply is that you could imagine that the cultural...
01:12:29.740
Okay, so let's say the core of the revelation is diffuse emotional activation, much of it
01:12:39.040
Okay, now the question is, what's the most basic form in which that could take perceptual
01:12:46.420
Okay, so the cultural narrative would be like a first order elaboration of the core of that
01:12:54.140
So then it's not surprising at all that that's what your mind would latch on to when it was
01:12:59.440
trying to clothe that emotion in perceptual reality.
01:13:04.240
Actually, so it turns out if you go first order and the basic level, most people will just see
01:13:10.040
So they won't even see the monster clothed and have all these details.
01:13:20.920
You know, the occipital lobes and the visual cortex responds to lines and basic shapes.
01:13:32.420
It's simply the brain says, look, I don't even care about the details.
01:13:43.140
Okay, so then as you explore, does it move up the visual...
01:13:47.800
So we know then that you have a part of the brain called the MT, the motor part of the
01:13:55.360
Then you have a part of the brain that has to do with, as we said, putting faces and
01:14:02.520
And so that comes as we move along the visual hierarchy.
01:14:05.840
And then finally you reach the vernica and the meaning part of the brain, the hippocampus,
01:14:10.160
and you go, my God, this is Freddy Krueger from Elm Street.
01:14:17.120
So, but most of the time, people will see these shadows and shapes and that's it.
01:14:25.360
Usually, the reason, the one reason for this is that usually when you don't have an identity,
01:14:30.160
it's even more scary because you have imagination.
01:14:32.200
Yes, of course, because you don't know what to do.
01:14:36.940
So then you imagine that adaptation would proceed in this manner.
01:14:41.400
So when it's only shapeless form, you have no idea what to do because there's no concrete
01:14:52.500
Okay, so now you can imagine using this in behavior therapy to deal with fears.
01:14:58.020
It's like, okay, first of all, because you're trying to get it to take form.
01:15:02.840
Because the more form it takes, the more delineated the strategy can be for dealing
01:15:07.700
So you're saying, okay, so first of all, you're doing a walk through the visual hierarchy.
01:15:13.180
You got just the shadows and the basic, and then you get the basic motion.
01:15:18.420
If you get depth, for example, you get color, V4 area in the brain with color, so you might
01:15:27.480
It takes a face, identity, and then you hook up the emotional part of the brain so it gets
01:15:31.800
The next thing you'd need above that would be a behavioral strategy.
01:15:36.440
So if this named and faced figure now makes itself manifest, what do you need to do?
01:15:42.980
And so, see, this is partly what you do if you're trying to treat someone for post-traumatic
01:15:47.860
stress disorder is you help them specify very clearly, so give form to, the nature of the
01:16:01.460
That's better than not knowing your enemy because that's even more terrifying.
01:16:04.640
But you want to lay out a behavioral strategy in relationship to that enemy that either
01:16:10.480
quells the threat, so how do you deal with a criminal, let's say, or like the optimal
01:16:17.380
strategy would be to take the enemy and to transform him into an ally, right?
01:16:22.200
That's like, that's the highest possible level of adaptation because who needs an enemy?
01:16:27.600
And partly what you're doing, like in therapy, you can go through someone's history and you
01:16:32.320
can see where they might have had a repeated traumatic experience, like in a relationship.
01:16:36.360
Then you have to find out what is it that's absent in their representation of relationship
01:16:41.600
that's exposing them time and time again to that threat because it means there's something
01:16:49.480
Because imagine this, imagine that you were selectively blind for, only for white vans that
01:16:59.500
When a woman faces an unexpected pregnancy, she often feels overwhelmed and alone.
01:17:04.720
Many initially consider abortion their only option, but thanks to donors like you, these
01:17:09.080
women may find their way to a pre-born network clinic where they receive the support that they
01:17:13.100
need to make an informed choice about their pregnancy.
01:17:15.900
At pre-born, women are offered compassionate care and a free ultrasound that allows them to
01:17:21.560
This combination of support and information helps many women choose to continue their pregnancies.
01:17:25.720
There are many women out there who are unaware of their options.
01:17:30.220
Acacia discovered that she was pregnant and felt completely overwhelmed and unsure about
01:17:34.680
She found her way to a pre-born network clinic where the staff provided her with supportive
01:17:39.880
Seeing her developing baby on the screen helped Acacia make the decision to continue with her pregnancy.
01:17:44.980
With your tax-deductible donation of $28, you can help provide one ultrasound for someone
01:17:51.660
Your contribution of any amount can make a real difference.
01:17:53.980
Last year alone, pre-born saw 67,000 babies saved from abortion.
01:17:58.140
If you'd like to support this work, please consider making a donation today.
01:18:04.480
That's pound 250, baby, or go to preborn.com slash Jordan.
01:18:10.960
It's not much of a deficit, but your recurring experience would be once every two years, you
01:18:20.660
get run over by a white fan, and you have no idea why.
01:18:24.380
Okay, so that's the account of the lack of your perception and the repeat experience.
01:18:29.960
Okay, so now you have someone who has a repetitive trauma.
01:18:32.260
You think, well, you've got a hole in your adaptive structure.
01:18:35.040
Okay, now you want to characterize that, so give it form, and you want to append a, it's
01:18:42.680
And that'll, that's what'll make the nightmare go away.
01:18:47.420
My experience clinically was that it all, it took very few repetitions of a repeating
01:18:59.000
The dream would, the, the dream would almost, my experience was that if you did that once,
01:19:04.620
the person wouldn't have the recurring dream anymore.
01:19:06.880
Right, right, and it's because you might say at some very deep level, they had conceptualized
01:19:12.780
themselves in that situation as nothing but a prey animal.
01:19:16.500
You do the same thing with exposure therapy, with agoraphobia, right, is you get people
01:19:20.560
to imagine what upsets them and then begin to confront it.
01:19:26.080
Right, so, and the generalization element isn't the specific strategy of confrontation, it's
01:19:32.380
the idea that they could be the one that confronts.
01:19:36.340
There's two points on that I want to, I want to go into that's really, very fascinating.
01:19:39.820
So, first of all, we should, we've shown that when you have a specific cultural narrative
01:19:44.840
for it and a name for it, right, the more terrifying and salient it becomes.
01:19:49.580
So, for example, if you live in Egypt and you say it's these evil genies, they come
01:19:53.100
at night, they choke you, they strangle you, they kill you, then you will, first of all,
01:19:59.740
So, so up to one third of more frequency to the experience.
01:20:07.060
So, we compared Egypt to Denmark, my home country.
01:20:09.580
So, we compared these two countries and it turns out when, in Denmark, by the way, people
01:20:12.980
say most of the time, it's just the brain, it's stress.
01:20:16.740
Like, there's no, there's no higher, like, explanation for it, right?
01:20:29.700
So, 50% of Danish people will say, I think, for Egyptians, will say, I will die from this.
01:20:39.900
It becomes, they say the paralysis lasts much longer.
01:20:46.640
So, it seems like through their cultural beliefs, the experience has become much more salient,
01:20:55.620
It's got elements of psychogenic epidemic to it, eh?
01:21:00.820
And we went to Italy and we looked at the Egyptians, the Italians with these terrifying
01:21:10.840
The Italians would also have these long episodes.
01:21:13.960
They would have them frequently and they would be extremely fearful.
01:21:17.500
So, it's like a contagious, maladaptive conceptualization.
01:21:23.220
And so, it turns out we have a theory for why that occurs.
01:21:26.360
So, imagine little Lisa living on this fictitious island in Simbuktu, for example.
01:21:31.360
And she has this conversation with her grandmother over, you know, dinner.
01:21:36.000
Let's say the grandmother says, at night, you will have this monster.
01:21:50.140
For the first time, she will have sleep paralysis now.
01:21:53.360
She will wake up the next day and she will have sleep paralysis again.
01:21:59.520
And then a month later, it's become chronic at this point.
01:22:03.600
And she'll go tell other people about it and they will have it too.
01:22:07.620
And then you might be asking, why is this the case?
01:22:11.480
We think that, first of all, the grandmother implanted these ideas into her brain about what sleep paralysis is.
01:22:18.260
When she's now sleeping in her bed, she will have nocturnal arousal, meaning the emotional part of the brain will be hyperactive during the REM stage.
01:22:27.460
And she will engage in this hyper-conformatory behavior where she will monitor any paralysis sensations, saying, is something holding me down?
01:22:40.180
And she's looking for an explanation for this now.
01:22:42.600
And then whenever, and because of her emotional centers being hyperactive, whenever she feels something, she'll go, my God, this is it.
01:22:51.900
And then the emotional, the hippocampus and all the narrative part of the brain will spill over into the experience.
01:22:58.600
And she will literally see whatever her grandmother was telling her.
01:23:01.680
Now you might say, why then does it happen a few days later?
01:23:07.860
And so a few days later, she will have anxiety and stress, which will predispose her.
01:23:12.220
We've shown that anxiety and stress predisposes you.
01:23:15.380
So she will have it again, two days later and three days later.
01:23:18.960
And at this point, she'll go, my God, I'm possessed.
01:23:24.340
This monster is, you know, chronically possessing me and it's coming after me.
01:23:29.760
And a funny thing is, too, it is a kind of possession.
01:23:33.680
Because the grandmother transmitted the spirit to her.
01:23:40.600
But at this point, what's interesting is that we've shown that people who have this episode,
01:23:45.540
they will have more anxiety and more PTSD-like symptoms from this, right?
01:23:50.220
So at this point, she might have these small teas of trauma of the monster coming and attacking you
01:24:00.520
Well, you can imagine how that would increase the probability that it would spread, too.
01:24:07.540
So she goes around and tells her friends about it.
01:24:09.460
And it turns out, then, that if you live in a culture like Egypt, it's twice as common versus Denmark.
01:24:17.340
So we said that for an individual person, you will have it three times more than the person who has sleep paralysis.
01:24:22.700
But beyond this, it's generally twice as common in cultures like Egypt and Italy and so forth.
01:24:31.220
So there's this element to it that's very, very interesting.
01:24:34.380
And I want to take you to an experiment that your colleague Rich did, Rich McNally, that sort of encapsulates all this.
01:24:40.600
So he showed that people who think that sleep paralysis is a space alien abduction,
01:24:45.660
so these guys will say, well, I was sleeping in my bed and this gray came down in a spaceship.
01:24:56.200
And obviously, we know from REM sleep, the hypothalamus and these parts of the brain are hyperactive.
01:25:03.180
So you have sexual arousal doing sleep paralysis.
01:25:06.580
So you have the monster coming down, taking their semen and all that.
01:25:14.940
But then at this point, what's interesting is that these guys, when they listen to the audio script of their encounters of themselves narrating,
01:25:23.940
so they'll listen to themselves saying, oh, I had this experience and this happened and that happened.
01:25:29.680
Their physiological reactions to that, their DSR, their sweating and their heart rate and blood pressure will be as profound as somebody who went to war.
01:25:38.680
So somebody with actual PTSD who went to a war situation, the physiological reactions they have is comparable to somebody who was sleeping in their beds and having sleep paralysis.
01:25:51.100
So for me, this shows me this might be one of the most interesting phenomena in the time of science.
01:25:54.280
Well, you can see, well, look, you know from psychotherapy that the simulations that produce the most psychophysiological response are the most curative.
01:26:05.280
So for example, when Edna Foa was treating people with PTSD, and I think she did this as well as anybody's ever done it,
01:26:13.180
she would have people, this is counterintuitive in some ways, so imagine that the trauma was rape instituted.
01:26:21.100
She'd have them bring the rape incident to mind in as much detail as possible, voluntarily.
01:26:30.540
And then she did psychophysiological measurements, and the participants who showed the highest levels of psychophysiological response to the reenactment were the ones that got better faster.
01:26:43.060
Now, that was still damn hard on them while they were doing it, but it makes perfect sense that the more hyperrealist simulation,
01:26:52.100
the more learning would be associated with it, obviously.
01:26:55.900
And you could see how that could be pathologized.
01:26:58.800
So then the question would be, this would be the tricky question for a therapist, is like,
01:27:04.900
well, what do you do with a client that has a repetitive alien abduction experience?
01:27:09.360
Because now that's a person who needs a strategy.
01:27:12.760
Well, so I developed a therapy for sleep paralysis.
01:27:17.880
So it's a four-step solution that I came up with.
01:27:20.340
So from my work around the world, I thought, I got to have some method to help these people, right?
01:27:31.280
And so one thing is that I noticed that, for example, prayer and meditation and prayer and thinking about positive things was helpful.
01:27:38.880
In a lot of instances, people would lay down and think about whatever God they're believing in, and that would actually help them.
01:27:44.840
So that got me thinking about the attention system and the emotional parts of the brain and how I can bring that into the experience, given that you have limited attentional capacities.
01:27:54.080
And so if you're lying there and thinking about ghosts because your emotional part of your brain, the amygdala, is hyperactive.
01:28:00.540
And you know the amygdala has a lot of projections to the visual parts of the brain, so it can tell you what to see in the world.
01:28:07.360
So it can spill over and penetrate the visual scene.
01:28:11.520
So I thought, how can I bring in the positive affect to the experience and make it more benign?
01:28:26.560
Meaning simply you say, look, this is not a monster.
01:28:39.300
And you close your eyes to just to filter any, you know, visual inputs.
01:28:44.840
So you're doing that with people when they're bringing the experience to mind?
01:29:00.920
You do the cognitive reappraisal saying, well, this is just your mind and brain playing tricks
01:29:06.860
Next step is emotional and psychological distancing.
01:29:11.600
You go, since it's just a brain, simply, given that it's common all around the world,
01:29:19.480
So you kind of distance yourself from the event.
01:29:24.720
Thirdly, this is where prayer and meditation and affect comes in.
01:29:28.800
And you put all your attention on a positive emotional object in your mind.
01:29:35.600
So you bring to mind, could be God, somebody who believes in God.
01:29:43.720
And you focus hyperattentively on that because then-
01:29:47.180
That's why Mary is an antidote to the demonic, right?
01:29:50.920
Because you bring the notion of mother and maternal care as the antithesis.
01:29:55.920
And so you bring in, so in that way, given the limited attentional abilities of the brain
01:30:01.440
and the frontal parietal regions and all that, so you focus intensely on this object, let's
01:30:08.300
And then fourthly, you meditate and meditate meaning you do a mindfulness kind of detachment.
01:30:16.460
You say, I feel spasmy and pain in my legs and I feel heavy, but I'm not going to do anything
01:30:26.340
And this turns out, these four steps, if you apply it during sleep paralysis, we did a pilot
01:30:31.640
study, a small study that showed it reduced sleep paralysis about 50%.
01:30:35.860
So that's a significant amount, but obviously we have to do more studies.
01:30:39.840
It's a very small study, but it's a first step.
01:30:42.180
It's the first empirical study on this as a treatment for sleep paralysis.
01:30:47.280
So I was very excited about that, but we need to do more research.
01:30:52.820
I walked the Via Dolorosa with Jonathan Paggio, and we were talking about its significance.
01:30:58.560
And so you could imagine that, imagine that one of the things that you have to do in life
01:31:03.920
is to, what do you say, reconcile yourself to the monstrous.
01:31:10.500
While walking the road of the crucifixion does that, because it enables you to voluntarily
01:31:15.600
simulate intense suffering in the face of malevolence.
01:31:21.540
Right now, you can imagine that because it's voluntary, rather than something that's imposed
01:31:26.320
on you, you're switching your framework from victim to accept, it's very strange, accepting
01:31:35.460
It's like turning around to confront the monster.
01:31:38.760
So you could imagine, we talked already about the figure of Mary, let's say, as, or briefly,
01:31:43.340
as the maternal, as the antithesis of the monstrous, that's pretty obvious.
01:31:48.760
But you could also imagine that practicing your ability to accept the reality of malevolence
01:31:56.680
That's a meta-strategy for dealing with the monstrous itself.
01:32:04.080
But you brought something to mind right now that I wanted to ask you about.
01:32:06.860
So obviously, I've seen your latest work on we wrestle with God and all this.
01:32:10.980
So do you have any thoughts about prophetic dreams, the kind of metaphors?
01:32:18.300
I wrote a little bit about it in the story of Noah.
01:32:29.140
But in the culturally and personally limited sense, in a way, Noah's as good a man as you
01:32:37.400
could expect some to be for the conditions of his time and place.
01:32:41.420
We know from the anthropological literature on elders that people who are singled out as
01:32:47.780
elders in, let's say, traditional communities are people who other people are motivated to
01:32:54.480
go to for advice when things aren't going well.
01:32:56.960
Okay, so now you might imagine, well, what sort of person would you have to be to be the
01:33:01.580
sort of person who people would go to for advice when things aren't going well?
01:33:05.840
Well, you'd have to establish a reputation of either having had things go well for you
01:33:11.600
so you could avoid the catastrophes, or having withstood a variety of catastrophes and still
01:33:20.100
Okay, so then that would make you a certain kind of person.
01:33:22.580
Well, the insistence in the story of Noah is that if your orientation is upward, your
01:33:34.320
If your intuition is valid, you're a prophet because you can see things coming when blind
01:33:40.600
So why wouldn't, like, there's no difference between forethought and prophecy except time
01:33:48.920
Okay, so you might say, well, a mature person is capable of forethought.
01:33:54.080
Okay, now if you are mature and maximally optimally configured in your moral aim, your capacity
01:34:04.500
You'd be able to see things coming long before other people.
01:34:08.780
And maybe some of that would reveal itself in, like, visions.
01:34:16.920
You know, imagine that your dream, imagine now you're the sort of person who's dispensed
01:34:23.060
Well, your dreams are still going to be doing something.
01:34:25.260
They're going to be concentrating on more sophisticated forms of anomaly.
01:34:33.500
Those are the sort of people that would be prophetic because, look, one of the ways of
01:34:38.760
being a prophet is just by looking at things that other people won't look at.
01:34:43.060
Because even in the landscape of media, if a story comes out about what's monstrous, most
01:34:53.960
Well, imagine instead you delve into it, like you delve into the character of the monster
01:35:09.820
Because there's also an insistence in hero mythology that you go to the heart of darkness.
01:35:18.540
And one reason that I bring this up is I was, I used to live in Egypt, as I told you,
01:35:23.960
and I was much inspired by the prophetic dreams of Joseph.
01:35:27.680
I thought they were very, they were very striking, the suns and sun and the moon bowing down and
01:35:33.740
So I think there's something, something very, very interesting about the whole, that realm
01:35:38.240
of explanation and how that sort of spills and trickles into narratives in religious scripture.
01:35:47.680
Well, like one of the, you might say that one of the strong functions of the religious
01:35:54.760
is the religious is a meta strategy for dealing with the monstrous.
01:36:02.740
Like how do we contend with not, you could imagine, how do I contend with my neighbor who's being
01:36:10.140
Well, then how do I contend with the class of neighbors who are being troublesome?
01:36:15.440
Well, then how do I contend with the fact that people can be malevolent?
01:36:19.760
Then how do I contend with the existence of malevolence itself?
01:36:24.260
So those are, you're getting deeper into the question with each of those iterations.
01:36:28.680
By the time you get to the point where your question is, how do I deal with the existence
01:36:38.920
So it was weirdly the case when I, when I was working as a clinician, when I was dealing
01:36:50.100
The language always became religious when we were talking about, you can't escape it
01:36:55.240
because if you've really been hurt by someone who really wanted to hurt you, you've been
01:37:01.300
touched by something like the spirit of malevolence.
01:37:04.000
And when you're having a discussion about the nature of the spirit of malevolence, the language
01:37:08.220
takes on like religious connotations and depth of its own accord.
01:37:14.820
So there's a phenomenon I'm sure you know of, it's temporal lobe epilepsy.
01:37:21.600
So they will have that in the, you know, selectively in the temporal lobes.
01:37:25.540
So they will, and this is the emotional part of the brain, become hyperactive.
01:37:29.340
And so they will see everything in the world as almost like they're living in a dream.
01:37:35.820
When I look at it, this pen right here, it's highly salient.
01:37:45.180
So it's just quite, and also Ramachandran and I wanted to study it more.
01:37:55.740
So these guys will become hyper-religious as well.
01:37:59.420
They will write all the time, you know, so they will develop this tendency to write.
01:38:03.280
And yeah, it's become hyper-poetic and quite an interesting phenomenon.
01:38:10.180
And some people have argued that in the temporal lobe, you have the god center of the brain,
01:38:15.660
In that region, that's where it all emanates in terms of the emotional landscape where that belief.
01:38:26.080
Well, one of the things, you know, if we have a further conversation at some point,
01:38:29.580
I'd like to talk about the, like, neurological conceptualization of the religious, by definition.
01:38:38.460
Like, because I think we're at a point in our understanding of neuroscience where we could have a conversation like that.
01:38:45.320
So, like, one of the hypotheses, for example, would be, imagine, I think that perceptions are the axioms of thought.
01:38:53.280
Okay, now, and so an axiom is a very deep presupposition.
01:38:58.140
You can imagine in any conceptual structure that there are shallow elements of the conception.
01:39:04.980
And then layers, kind of like the visual system.
01:39:07.640
Layers, and that there are axiomatic elements of the conceptual system.
01:39:13.000
If an axiomatic element is accidentally shifted, you're traumatized.
01:39:22.200
But as the level of depth of the inquiry maximizes, the inquiry becomes more religious in its nature.
01:39:36.000
And so, then you could imagine that the salience of the investigation magnifies in proportion to its depth.
01:39:43.960
And so, that would account for the experiential element of awe, for example.
01:39:48.760
So, that's something that would be very entertaining to discuss technically.
01:39:54.300
And I think with the whole temporal lobe epilepsy, right?
01:39:56.920
So, the whole landscape becomes hyper, you know.
01:39:59.700
So, the regions, we talked about the fusiform face area before.
01:40:03.440
The regions from the cluster of cells in the fusiform and the emotional part of the brain become hyperconnected and hyperactive.
01:40:13.000
So, that's why when you look at a dry object like a pen, it becomes hypercosmic and emotional and spiritual, right?
01:40:24.560
So, if you have what's called Cotard syndrome, everything in the world is dead.
01:40:31.700
Everything in the world is almost like depression, in fact.
01:40:36.400
And you look at yourself in the mirror and you go, my God, I am dead.
01:40:41.120
And the doctor will say, what do you mean you are dead?
01:40:46.780
And then they'll say, what about if I take a needle and poke you and are you dead?
01:41:05.140
So, it shows you that, you know, something intriguing is going on in the brain.
01:41:09.680
When these centers are hyperactive, everything becomes salient.
01:41:13.000
If they're underactive, you know, you are dead.
01:41:16.720
You know, in fact, if that part of the brain, the face area in the brain and the emotional
01:41:20.760
part of the brain is hyperactive, there's another syndrome called Fregoli syndrome, where
01:41:25.600
you go around and you say, everybody in the landscape looks like my Uncle Joe, for example.
01:41:30.120
So, you go around and say, well, this looks like my Uncle Joe and this looks like my Uncle
01:41:35.320
And the reason is that you have hyper-emotionality.
01:41:40.080
Because of that vision, the emotion part of the brain and the face part of the brain are
01:41:50.080
And then you feel like, I shouldn't have emotions.
01:41:53.740
I shouldn't have emotions when I go around in the landscape and looking at people.
01:41:58.720
Therefore, your brain jumps to these absurd conclusions and go, you know, these are all
01:42:05.200
We should close with this and we'll move to the Daily Wear section.
01:42:07.860
But you see the same thing with the onset of paranoid schizophrenia is that, say, someone
01:42:12.980
is watching television and part of the speech becomes hyper-emotionally significant, right?
01:42:22.800
And the more intelligent schizophrenics are more likely to become paranoid, by the way,
01:42:28.200
because they build up the conceptual structures around the perceptual anomaly.
01:42:32.680
But it's the perceptual emotional anomaly that's the core of the pathology.
01:42:40.520
Well, because you get this emotional hyper-response.
01:42:47.720
It's like, well, it's like this is, it's particularly significant to me.
01:43:03.120
There's no denying the reality of the experience, right?
01:43:06.460
So, then the paranoid conspiracy, let's say, is overlaid on top of that as an explanation
01:43:12.400
for something that can't be challenged because it's so visceral, right?
01:43:16.940
And it is dreamlike in the sense that you just described.
01:43:19.780
And one of the things you also alluded to was that we actually base our sense of reality
01:43:25.680
So, it's hyper-real if everything's over-valence and it's dead if nothing has significance.
01:43:43.280
And you're working really in the same area in the Harvard Department of Psychology that
01:43:50.340
And so, one of the things I'd like to talk to you about is your experience there and your
01:43:57.180
So, let's do that for half an hour on the Daily Wire side.
01:44:00.520
So, all of you who are watching and listening, you can, well, you can continue your investigation
01:44:06.960
into exactly the topics that we described today.
01:44:10.020
Obviously, at Peterson Academy, because I'm lecturing there and my guest is lecturing there,
01:44:19.020
And so, that's one of the newer courses in our offerings.
01:44:21.320
And so, if you're fascinated by this sort of thing, it's so useful to know the anatomy and
01:44:27.320
the neuroscience, as I said, because it gives you much deeper insights into, well, the nature
01:44:33.600
of the problems that you might encounter and also into the nature of their, of what?
01:44:42.040
And we're putting a tremendous amount together right now on the scientific and cultural front
01:44:47.380
in relationship to the overlap between brain function, neurochemistry, physiology, and behavior,
01:44:57.960
And so, the courses that, well, this Introduction to Neuroscience course is one that focuses
01:45:06.020
So, in any case, join us on the Daily Wire side for another half an hour.
01:45:10.120
We'll talk about the state of the modern university, focusing in this case on Harvard and the
01:45:15.400
Department of Psychology there, although you've been at other universities too, and we can bring
01:45:24.920
Thanks to the film crew here today in Scottsdale and to the Daily Wire for making this possible.
01:45:29.220
And finally, to all of you for your time and attention.